Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 14, Issue 6, December 2001, Pages 1486-1492
NeuroImage

Rapid Communication
Access to Deductive Logic Depends on a Right Ventromedial Prefrontal Area Devoted to Emotion and Feeling: Evidence from a Training Paradigm

https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0930Get rights and content

Abstract

Doesthe human capacity for access to deductive logic depend on emotion and feeling? With positron emission tomography, we compared the brain networks recruited by two groups of subjects who were either able or not able to shift from errors to logical responses in a deductive reasoning task. They were scanned twice while performing the same task, before and after a training session. The error-to-logical shift occurred in a group that underwent logicoemotional training but not in the other group, trained in logic only—a “cold” kind of training. The intergroup comparison pointed out that access to deductive logic involved a right ventromedial prefrontal area known to be devoted to emotion and feeling.

References (34)

  • S.W. Anderson et al.

    Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy

    Science

    (1997)
  • G. Bush et al.

    Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2000)
  • C.S. Carter et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance

    Science

    (1998)
  • A.R. Damasio

    Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain

    (1994)
  • A.R. Damasio

    The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness

    (1999)
  • H. Damasio et al.

    The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient

    Science

    (1994)
  • F.N. Dempster et al.

    Interference and Inhibition in Cognition

    (1995)
  • R. Descartes

    Rules for the Direction of the Mind

    (1628/1961)
  • M. D'Esposito et al.

    Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from event-related fMRI studies

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • J.St.B.T. Evans

    Reasoning with negatives

    Br. J. Psychol.

    (1972)
  • J.St.B.T. Evans

    Biases in Human Reasoning

    (1989)
  • J.St.B.T. Evans

    Matching bias in conditional reasoning

    Thinking Reasoning

    (1998)
  • K.J. Friston et al.

    Spatial registration and normalization of images

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1995)
  • V. Goel et al.

    The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning

    NeuroReport

    (1997)
  • Cited by (72)

    • Inhibitory control and the understanding of buoyancy from childhood to adulthood

      2021, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Handling the marbles with hands could also be used as a pedagogical intervention to improve buoyancy judgment. Future studies should also investigate whether a metacognitive intervention centered on learning to identify the intuitive conception and to inhibit it, which has proven to be efficient in overcoming reasoning biases (Houdé et al., 2000, 2001; Lubin, Lanoë, Pineau, & Rossi, 2012; Rossi, Lubin, Lanoë, & Pineau, 2012), could improve scientific reasoning. The results of the current study are in line with previous findings (Potvin & Cyr, 2017; Potvin, Masson, et al., 2015), indicating that children and adolescents must inhibit their spontaneous intuitive conception that “bigger objects sink more” in order to correctly compare the buoyancy of objects in a context where the smaller object is denser.

    • Neurocognitive processes underlying heuristic and normative probability judgments

      2020, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Brain imaging is a suitable complement to the encompassing behavioral literature regarding these issues, as the arguments for isolable judgment processes imply that they should be separable also in terms of underlying brain function (Evans & Stanovich, 2013; Keren & Schul, 2009). Although associative (Binder & Desai, 2011) and analytical (De Martino, Kumaran, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006; De Neys, Vartanian, & Goel, 2008; Houdé et al., 2001; Leroux et al., 2009; Mega, Gigerenzer, & Volz, 2015) thinking have been investigated with brain imaging methods before, neurophysiological evidence for the separation of the assumed processes is lacking (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). We therefore aimed to test core assumptions of dual-process theory using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while participants judged variations of the emblematic Linda example: (i) if the conjunction fallacy is due to attribute substitution to a representativeness heuristic, incorrect probability judgments should involve the same brain regions that underlie similarity judgments; (ii) if avoidance of the conjunction fallacy is governed by a separate process characterized by cognitive decoupling, then such judgments should involve a different or additional set of brain regions related to executive control (Evans & Stanovich, 2013).

    • Ethical views and considerations

      2020, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
    • Is inhibition involved in overcoming a common physics misconception in mechanics?

      2015, Trends in Neuroscience and Education
      Citation Excerpt :

      The concept of inhibition is particularly interesting for science learning, especially with regard to students׳ misconceptions, because it could come into play when overcoming a bad response to select a good response [86]. Recently, some results indicated that inhibition would be involved in logical reasoning [20,44,45], in mathematical reasoning about perimeter and area [79], and during a classic Piagetian number conservation task [43]. For these specific tasks, it appears that the areas of the brain related to inhibition are more activated when participants are able to provide an accurate answer.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    To whom correspondence should be adressed at Groupe d'Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle (UMR 6095), Université Paris 5, Sorbonne, 46 Rue Saint-Jacques, 75005 Paris, France. Fax: 33 (0) 1 40 46 29 93. E-mail: [email protected].

    View full text