Skip to main content

The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information

  • Cognitive Aspects of Spatial Acquisition
  • Conference paper
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Computer Science ((LNCS,volume 1329))

Abstract

Research on direct sources of information for the perception and cognition of environmental distance is reviewed. Environmental distances are relatively large and cannot be perceived in entirety from a single place.Directly-acquired knowledge of environmental distance is based on the sensorimotor apprehension of information fromthe body or from the environment during locomotion. The paper is organized around the idea that distance knowledge is derived from multiple, partially redundant information sources. After briefly discussing general issues, I review literature on direct sources of distance information,including environmental features, travel time, and travel effort. Theories of how these sources provide information about distance are considered. To date, only information from environmental features has received much empirical support as a direct source of distance knowledge, particularly to the extent that the features segment routes into vistas. Key weaknesses in the empirical literatureare discussed, and importantresearch directions are identified.

An expanded version of this manuscript, including a comprehensive reference list, is available from the author.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allen, G. L. (1981) A developmental perspective on the effectsof “subdividing” macrospatial experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 7:120–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. L., and Kirasic, K. C. (1985) Effects of the cognitiveorganization of route knowledge on judgments of macrospatial distance. Memory and Cognition 13:218–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, J. C. (1970) Psychophysical analysis of visual space.NewYork: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bamford, C. L. (1988) The effect of effort on distance estimation. Unpublished master's thesis, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A., ed. (1992) Cognitive models of psychological time. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A., and Reed, M. A. (1978) Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 4:656–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brimberg, J. (1992) A new distance function for modeling travel distances in a transportation network. Transportation Science 26:129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buffardi, L. (1971) Factors affecting the filled-duration illusion in the auditory, tactual, and visual modalities. Perception and Psychophysics 10:292–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, P. (1978) Time cognition and urban travel behavior. Geografiska Annaler 60B:107–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadwallader, M. T. (1976) Cognitive distance in intraurban space. In Environmental knowing, eds. G. T. Moore and R. G. Golledge, pp. 316–324. Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canter, D., and Tagg, S. K. (1975) Distance estimation in cities.Environment and Behavior 7:59–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., and Cooper, P. (1962) New phenomena in apparent duration, distance, and speed. Nature 196:1233–1234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R., Baldwin, L. M., and Sherman, R C. (1978) Cognitive maps of a naturalistic setting. Child Development 49:1216–1218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coshall, J. T. (1985) Urban consumers' cognitions of distance. Geografiska Annaler 67B:107–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Couclelis, H., Golledge, R. G., Gale, N., and Tobler, W. (1987) Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition. Journal of Environmental Psychology 7:99–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, K. W., and Isard, W. (1961) A note on a generalized concept of effective distance. Behavioral Science 6:308–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M., and Stea, D. (1977) Maps in minds: Reflections on cognitive mapping. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, G., and Bratfisch, O. (1965) Subjective distance and emotional involvement:, A psychologicalmechanism. Acta Psychologica 24:430–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ewing, G. O. (1981) On the sensitivity of conclusions about the basesof cognitive distance. Professional Geographer 33:311–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., and Golledge, R. G. (1990) A minimal representation for dead-reckoning navigation: updating the homing vector. Geographical Analysis 22:326–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., and Stimson, R. J. (1997) Spatial behavior: A geographic perspective. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G., and Zannaras, G. (1973) Cognitive approaches to the analysis of human spatial behavior. In Environment and cognition, ed. W. H. Ittelson, pp. 59–94. New York: Seminar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1988) The vicissitudes of ecological phenomena in environmentbehavior research: On the failure to replicate the “angularity effect”. Environment and Behavior 20:92–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, J. F., Norton, L. M., and Klein, C. A. (1986) Children's distance estimates in a large-scale environment: A search forthe route angularity effect. Environment and Behavior 18:533–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirtle, S. C., and Jonides, J. (1985) Evidence of hierarchiesin cognitive maps. Memory and Cognition 13:208–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirtle, S. C., and Mascolo, M. F. (1991) Heuristics in distance estimation. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., and Mah, W. A. (1982) Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitudes. Cognitive Psychology 14:328–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., Cicinelli, J. G., Doherty, S., and Pellegrino, J. W. (1990) Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior 22:19–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., Pick, H. L., and Fariello, C. P. (1974) Cognitive maps in children and men. Child Development 45:707–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Künnapas, T. (1960) Scales for subjective distance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology l:187–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Collins, A., and Wardell, J. (1987) Exploring environments by hand or foot: Time-based heuristics for encoding distance in movement space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13:606–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacEachren, A. M. (1980) Travel time as the basis for cognitive distance. Professional Geographer 32:30–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D. R. (1988) Route information and travel time as bases for the perception and cognition of environmental distance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D. R. (1991) The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity. Journal of Environmental Psychology 11:101–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D. R. (in press) A new framework for understanding the acquistion of spatial knowledge in large-scale environments. In Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems, eds. R.G. Golledge and M. J. Egenhofer. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D. R., and Freundschuh, S. M. (1995) Sources of spatial knowledge and their implications for GIS: An introduction. Geographical Systems 2:169–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nasar, J. L., Valencia, H., Omar, Z. A., Chueh, S., and Hwang, J. (1985) Out of sight further from mind: Destination visibility and distance perception. Environment and Behavior 17:627–639.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, N., and Liben, L. S. (1982) Barrier effects in the cognitive maps of children and adults. Journal of ExperimentalChild Psychology 34:46–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J. J., Lockman, J. L., and Pick, H. L. (1980) The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout. Perception and Psychophysics 28:185–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, W. J., and Staplin, L. J. (1980) Reference points in spatial cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6:516–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadalla, E. K., and Magel, S. G. (1980) The perception of traversed distance. Environment and Behavior 12:65–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadalla, E. K., and Montello, D. R. (1989) Remembering changes in direction. Environment and Behavior 21:346–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadalla, E. K., Staplin, L. J., and Burroughs, W. J. (1979) Retrieval processes in distance cognition. Memory andCognition 7:291–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Säisä, J., Svensson-Garling, A., Gärling, T., and Lindberg, E. (1986) Intraurban cognitive distance: The relationship between judgmentsof straight-line distances, travel distances, and travel times. Geographical Analysis 18:167–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmaso, P., Baropi, M. R., Job, R., and Peron, E. M. (1983) Schematic information, attention, and memory for places. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9:263–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. D. (1984) The relationship between the pleasingness of landmarks and the judgment of distanceincognitivemaps. Journal of Environmental Psychology 4:229–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, D. L. (1963) New concept: “Subjective distance”. Journal of Retailing 39:1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndyke, P. W., and Hayes-Roth, B. (1982) Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology 14:560–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, D., Tsal, Y., Moses, M., and Shahar, I. (1994) The role of segmentation in prospective and retrospective time estimation processes. Memory and Cognition 22:344–351. *** DIRECT SUPPORT *** A0008169 00006

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Stephen C. Hirtle Andrew U. Frank

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Montello, D.R. (1997). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information. In: Hirtle, S.C., Frank, A.U. (eds) Spatial Information Theory A Theoretical Basis for GIS. COSIT 1997. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 1329. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-63623-4_57

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-63623-4_57

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-63623-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-540-69616-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics