Skip to main content

Presence: Form, Content and Consciousness

  • Chapter
Book cover Immersed in Media

Abstract

In this chapter we present a rather wide-ranging perspective on presence as a central, characterizing feature of conscious mental life. After clarifying what we mean by presence in the first section, Sect. 3.2 discusses the implications of this for measurement. In Sect. 3.3, we consider the importance of media form for the sense of presence, before moving on in Sect. 3.4 to the relationship between presence and the sense of self considered in evolutionary terms. Section 3.5 deals specifically with attention, viewing presence as a reflection of attentional focus. Our aim is to convey the big picture about presence: what it is, what it’s for, how it evolved, what it is determined by and the effects it can have.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Distal attribution is a related term. Loomis suggested in the very first issue of Presence journal (Loomis 1992) that distal attribution results when afference is lawfully related to efference (after White 1970) and that attribution to self occurs when they are unrelated.

  2. 2.

    Damasio refers to this as the “autobiographical self ”. But because of its intrinsic dependence on extended consciousness, and because it consists of more than autobiographical memories and the self-conscious idea of self, we prefer to call this third layer the “extended self ”.

  3. 3.

    Alpha waves indicate activity of the visual cortex in an idle state.

References

  • Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioural and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baños, R. M., Botella, C., Guerrero, B., Liaño, V., Alcañiz, M., & Rey, B. (2005). The third pole of the sense of presence: Comparing virtual and imagery spaces. PsychNology, 3(1), 90–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomeo, P. (2002). The relationship between visual perception and visual mental imagery: A reappraisal of the neuropsychological evidence. Cortex, 38, 357–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biocca, F. (1992). Communication within virtual reality: Creating a space for research. Journal of Communication, 42(4), 5–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biocca, F. (2003, May 7). Can we resolve the book, the physical reality, and the dream state problems? A three pole model of presence. Presentation at EU presence research conference, Venice, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biocca, F., Kim, J., & Choi, Y. (2001). Visual touch in virtual environments: An exploratory study of presence, multimodal interfaces, and cross-modal sensory illusions. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual environments, 10(3), 247–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brett, J. F., & eWalle, D. V. (1999). Goal orientation and goal content as predictors of performance in a training program. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(6), 863–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., James, K., & Citera, M. (1994). A goal hierarchy model of personality, motivation and leadership. Research in Organizational Behavior, 15, 267–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cytowic, R. (1989). Synaesthesia. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1994). Decartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Penguin Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. San Diego: Harcourt Brace and Co, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, R. J. (1999). Feeling the neurobiological self. Nature, 401, 847–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty, M. G. (1999). A watched pot: How we experience time. New York: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Floridi, L. (2004, October 13–15). Exploring the informational nature of presence. Opening invited keynote address at 7th annual international workshop on presence 2004. Polytechnic University of Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J., Avons, S. E., Pearson, D. E., Meddis, R., & IJsselsteijn, W. A. (2000). Using behavioural realism to estimate presence: A study of the utility of postural responses to motion-stimuli. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 9(2), 149–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J., Lessiter, J., Keogh, E., Bond, F. W., & Chapman, K. (2004). Relaxation island: Virtual, and really relaxing. In Proceedings of presence 2004, Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J., Lessiter, J., Pugh, K. & Keogh, E. (2005). When presence and emotion are related, and when they are not. In Proceedings of presence 2005, London, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. (1998). Eye and brain: The psychology of seeing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heeter, C. (2003). Reflections on real presence by a virtual person. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 12(4), 335–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. (1992). A history of the mind. New York: Simon and Shuster.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R., & Frantz, C. M. (1997). The impact of trauma on meaning: From meaningless world to meaningful life. In M. Power & C. R. Brewin (Eds.), The transformation of meaning in psychological therapies (pp. 91–106). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. T. (2007, October 25–27). Presence as external versus internal experience: How form, user, style, and content factors produce presence from the Inside. In Proceedings of presence 2007, 10th annual international workshop on presence, Barcelona, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kåver, A., & Nilsonne, Å. (2002). Dialektisk beteendeterapi vid emotionellt instabil personlighetsstörining – Teori, strategi och teknik. Sweden: Natur och Kultur (in Swedish only).

    Google Scholar 

  • Laarni, J., Ravaja, N., Saari, T. & Hartmann, T. (2004, October). Personality-related differences in subjective presence. In Proceedings of presence 2004. Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee S., Gerard G. J., Rizzo, A., & Park, H.(2004, October). Formation of spatial presence: By form or content? In Proceedings of Presence 2004. Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. & Ditton, T. (1997). Presence: At the heart of it all. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 3(2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, J. M. (1992). Distal attribution and presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1(1), 113–119.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (1999). The hint half guessed. Scientific American, 11, 184–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mikropoulos, T. A., Tzimas, E. & Dimou, G. L. (2004, October). Objective presence measures through electric brain activity. In Proceedings of Presence 2004. Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohayon, M. M., Zulley, J., Guilleminault, C., & Smirne, S. (1999). Prevalence and pathologic associations of sleep paralysis in the general population. Neurology, 52, 1194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain. New York: William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia – A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regenbrecht, H., Schubert, T., & Friedmann, F. (1998). Measuring the sense of presence and its relations to fear of heights in virtual environments. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 10(3), 233–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G. & Waterworth, J. A. (2003). Presence and the self: A cognitive neuroscience approach. Presence-Connect, 3(3), posted 7 April 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G., Wiederhold, B., & Molinari, E. (Eds.). (1999). Virtual environments in clinical psychology and neuroscience. Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G., Waterworth, J. A., & Waterworth, E. L. (2004). The layers of presence: A bio-cultural approach to understanding presence in natural and mediated environments. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 7(4), 402–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riva, G., Mantovani, F., Waterworth, E. L., & Waterworth, J. A. (2015). Intention, action, self and other: An evolutionary model of presence. In M. Lombard, F. Biocca, J. Freeman, W. IJsselsteijn, & R. J. Schaevitz (Eds.), Immersed in media: Telepresence theory, measurement & technology (pp. 73–99). Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roche, M. S., & McConkey, K. M. (1990). Absorption: Nature, assessment and correlates. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 59(1), 91–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sas, C., & O’Hare, G. M. P. (2003). Presence equation: An investigation into cognitive factors underlying presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 12(5), 523–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schier, M. A. (2000). Changes in EEG alpha power during simulated driving: a demonstration. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 155–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater, M. (2002). Presence and the sixth sense. Presence: Teleoperators, and Virtual Environments, 11(4), 435–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater, M. (2003). A note on presence terminology. Presence-Connect, 3(3).

    Google Scholar 

  • Steuer, J. (1992). Defining virtual reality: Dimensions determining telepresence. Journal of Communication, 42(4), 73–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Usoh M., Arthur K., Whitton M., Bastos R., Steed A., Brooks F., & Slater M. (1999). The visual cliff revisited: A virtual presence study on locomotion. In 2nd international workshop on presence, April 6–7, University of Essex, Colchester, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velmans, M. (2000). Understanding consciousness. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A. (1984). The influence of variations in cognitive processing on the perception of time. PhD thesis, University of Hertfordshire, UK. Available through the British Lending Library (Accession no. D50267/84).

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A. (1996, November). VR for animals. In Proceedings of ciber@RT’96, Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A. (1997). Creativity and sensation: The case for synaesthetic media. Leonardo, 30(4), 327–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A. & Fällman, D. (2003, September). The reality helmet: Transforming the experience of being-in-the-world. In Part 2 of Proceedings of HCI 2003, Bath, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, E. L. & Waterworth J. A. (2000a, March). Using a telescope in a cave: Presence and absence in educational VR. Presented at Presence 2000: Third international workshop on presence, Delft, Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, E. L., & Waterworth, J. A. (2000b). Presence and absence in educational VR: The role of perceptual seduction in conceptual learning. Themes in Education, 1(1), 7–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, E. L., & Waterworth, J. A. (2001). Focus, locus and sensus: The 3 dimensions of virtual experience. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 4(2), 203–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A., & Waterworth, E. L. (2003a). Being and time: Judged presence and duration as a function of media form. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 12(5), 495–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A. & Waterworth, E. L. (2003b). The meaning of presence. Presence-Connect, 3(3), posted 13 February 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A., & Waterworth, E. L. (2006). Presence as a dimension of communication: Context of use and the person. In G. Riva, M. T. Anguera, B. K. Wiederhold, & F. Mantovani (Eds.), From communication to presence: Cognition, emotions and culture towards the ultimate communicative experience (Festschrift in honor of Luigi Anolli). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A., Waterworth, E. L. & Westling, J. (2002, October 9–11). Presence as performance: The mystique of digital participation. In Proceedings of presence 2002. Porto, Portugal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A., Lund, A., & Modjeska, D. (2003a). Experiential design of shared information spaces. In K. Höök, D. Benyon, & A. Munro (Eds.), Designing information spaces: The social navigation approach. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, E. L., Häggkvist, M., Jalkanen, K., Olsson, S., Waterworth, J. A., & Wimelius, H. (2003b). The exploratorium: An environment to explore your feelings. Psychnology, 1(3), 189–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterworth, J. A., Waterworth, E. L., Mantovani, F., & Riva, G. (2010). On feeling (the) present: An evolutionary account of the sense of presence in physical and electronically-mediated environments. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17(1–2), 167–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, B. W. (1970). Perceptual findings with the vision-substitution system. IEEE Transactions on Man-machine Systems, MMS, 11, 54–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John A. Waterworth .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Waterworth, J.A., Waterworth, E.L., Riva, G., Mantovani, F. (2015). Presence: Form, Content and Consciousness. In: Lombard, M., Biocca, F., Freeman, J., IJsselsteijn, W., Schaevitz, R. (eds) Immersed in Media. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-10189-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-10190-3

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics