Skip to main content
Log in

Connectionism, classical cognitive science and experimental psychology

  • Published:
AI & SOCIETY Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Classical symbolic computational models of cognition are at variance with the empirical findings in the cognitive psychology of memory and inference. Standard symbolic computers are well suited to remembering arbitrary lists of symbols and performing logical inferences. In contrast, human performance on such tasks is extremely limited. Standard models donot easily capture content addressable memory or context sensitive defeasible inference, which are natural and effortless for people. We argue that Connectionism provides a more natural framework in which to model this behaviour. In addition to capturing the gross human performance profile, Connectionist systems seem well suited to accounting for the systematic patterns of errors observed in the human data. We take these arguments to counter Fodor and Pylyshyn's (1988) recent claim that Connectionism is, in principle, irrelevant to psychology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1976).The Psychology of Memory, New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932).Remembering: a Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow, D. G. and Norman, D. A. (1975). Some principles of memory schemata. In D. G. Bobrow and A. M. Collins (eds)Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. N. Y.: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1970). Analysis of a mnemonic device.American Scientist, 58, 496–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R. and Franks, J. J. (1972). Sentence memory: a constructive versus interpretive approach.Cognitive Psychology, 3, 193–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D. and Johnson, M. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: some investigations of comprehension and recall.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D. and Johnson, M. K. (1973). Considerations of some problems of comprehension. In W. G. Chase (ed.)Visual Information Processing, pp. 389–392. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D. and McCarrell, N. S. (1975). A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension: some thoughts about what it means to comprehend. In W. B. Weimar and D. S. Palermo (eds)Cognition and Symbolic Processes, pp. 189–229. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N. and Oaksford, M. R. (in press). Autonomy, implementation and cognitive architecture: a reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn.Cognition.

  • Cheng, P. W. and Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schema.Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, P. W., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E. and Oliver, L. M. (1986). Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches in training deductive reasoning.Cognitive Psychology, 18, 293–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. and Haviland, S. E. (1977). Comprehension and the given-new contract. In Freedle, R. O. (ed.)Discourse Production and Comprehension, Volume 1, pp. 1–40. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M. and Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 8, 240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Kleer, J. (1986). Extending the ATMS.Artificial Intelligence, 28, 163–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (1982).The Psychology of Deductive Reasoning. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. and Garrett, M. F. (1974)The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975).The Language of Thought. New York: Thomas Crowell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1980). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology.Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 3, 63–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983).Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1987).Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. and Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis.Cognition, 28, 3–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griggs, R. A. and Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason's selection task.British Journal of Psychology, 73, 407–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (1986).Change in View. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E. (1981). Implementing semantic networks in parallel hardware. Chapter 6 inParallel Models of Associative Memory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E. (1987). Learning distributed representations of concepts. InProceedings of the Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1987.

  • Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L. and Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed Representations. InParallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Volume 1:Foundations. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Israel, D. J. (1980). What's wrong with non-monotonic logic? InProceedings of AAAI-80, 1980, pp. 99–101.

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Wason, P. C. (1970). A theoretical analysis of insight into a reasoning task.Cognitive Psychology, 1, 134–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Steedman, M. J. (1978). The psychology of syllogisms.Cognitive Psychology, 10, 64–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1986). Reasoning without logic. Chapter 1 in T. Myers, K. Brown and B. McGonigle (eds)Reasoning and Discourse Processes, pp. 13–51. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1970).The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd edition, Chicago, Illinois: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of research programmes. In I. Lakatos and A. Musgrave (eds)Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge, pp. 91–196. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, P. H. and Norman, D. A. (1977).Human Information Processing, 2nd Edition. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorayne, H. and Lucas, J. (1974).The Memory Book. New York: Stein and Day.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1976).Cognitive Development. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masterman, M. (1970). The nature of a paradigm. In Lakatos, I. and Musgrave, A. (eds)Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. (1980). Circumscription: a form of non-monotonic reasoning.Artificial Intelligence, 13, 27–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. and Hinton, G. E. (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing. InParallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Volume 1:Foundations. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, D. (1986). A Critique of Pure Reason. Technical Report, Department of Computer Science, Yale University, June, 1986.

  • Miller, G. A. and Selfridge, J. A. (1950). Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material.American Journal of Psychology, 63, 176–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two, or, some limits on our capacity for processing information.Psychological Review, 63, 81–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1975). Frame-system theory. In R. Schank and B. L. Nash-Webber (eds)Theoretical Issues in Natural Language Processing. Cambridge, Mass, June 10–13, 1975.

  • Murphy, G. L. and Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence.Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. and Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes.Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. and Bobrow, D. G. (1976). On the role of active memory processes in perception and cognition. In C. N. Cofer (ed.)The Structure of Human Memory. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. and Bobrow, D. G. (1979). Descriptions: an intermediate stage in memory retrieval.Cognitive Psychology, 11, 107–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M. R. (1988). Cognition and Inquiry: The Pragmatics of Conditional Reasoning. PhD Thesis, Centre for Cognitive Science, University of Edinburgh.

  • Oaksford, M. R. and Stenning, K. (1988). Process and Pragmatics in Reasoning with Conditionals containing Negated Constituents. Presented to the International Conference on Thinking, University of Aberdeen, August, 1988.

  • Peterson, L. R. and Peterson, M. (1959). Short-term retention of individual items.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard, P. and Evans, J. S. B. T. (1981). The effects of prior belief in reasoning: an associational interpretation.British Journal of Psychology, 72, 73–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1974). The ‘corroboration’ of theories. In P. A. Schilpp (ed.)The Philosophy of Karl Popper, Volume I, pp. 221–240. La Salle, Illinois: The Open Court Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: a critique of mental imagery.Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984).Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Montgomery, Vermont: Bradford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiter, R. (1985). On reasoning by default. In R. Brachman and H. Levesque (eds)Readings in Knowledge Representation. Los Altos, California: Morgan Kaufman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L. and Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. Chapter 14 in J. L. McClelland, and D. E. Rumelhart (eds)Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, Volume 2:Psychological and Biological Processes, pp. 7–57. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for word, sentences and pictures.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 156–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K., Shepherd, M. and Levy, J. (1988). On the construction of representations for individuals from descriptions in text.Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, 129–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K. and Levy, J. (1988) Knowledge-rich solutions to the binding problem: a simulation of some human computational mechanisms.Knowledge Based Systems, 1, 143–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1966) Reasoning. In Foss, B. (ed.)New Horizons in Psychology. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972).Psychology of Reasoning: Structure and Content. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. and Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. N. and Gibson, W. B. (1962).How to Develop an Exceptional Memory. Chilton: Radnor, Pa.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Oaksford, M., Chater, N. & Stenning, K. Connectionism, classical cognitive science and experimental psychology. AI & Soc 4, 73–90 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01889765

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01889765

Keywords

Navigation