Skip to main content
Log in

No anticipation–no action: the role of anticipation in action and perception

  • Review
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper reviews psychophysical evidence for the existence and the nature of two types of anticipation in goal-oriented action. The first one relates to attained changes of the perceptual world, thus to action goals. These anticipations determine appropriate motor output. We argue that goal codes do not only serve as a reference unit, against which currently produced behavioral effects are compared. Rather voluntary actions appear to be planned literally in terms of intended behavioral effects. The second type of anticipation relates to the environmental conditions that have to be met to bring an intended effect into being. These anticipations serve to trigger selected actions, when appropriate execution conditions are encountered. Altogether, the behavioral evidence portrays a remarkable automaticity of goal-oriented action. Once a goal exists (wherever it might come from), corresponding efferent output is generated and executed under appropriate conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Imitation has also been reported when observed and imitated movements cannot be directly linked via the same perceptual channel. For example, newborns imitate the seen facial expressions of adults, although the babies have not acquired a visual representation of their own facial expressions (Meltzoff and Moore 1977). This has been explained by the assumption of inborn supramodal representations of certain actions’ consequences (cf. Heyes 2001 for further discussion of this issue).

References

  • Ach N (1905) Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams JA (1971) A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Mot Behav 3:111–149

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W (2001) Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol 106:3–22

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Braver TS, Barch DM (2006) Extracting core components of cognitive control. Trends cogn Sci 10:529–532

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butz MV, Hoffmann J (2002) Anticipations control behavior: animal behavior in an anticipatory learning classifier system. Adapt Behav 10:75–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Naccache L, Le Clec HG, Koechlin E, Mueller M, Dehaene-Lambertz G, van de Moortele PF, Le Bihan D (1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395:597–600

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget MP, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4:324–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donders FV (1868/1969) On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychol 30:412–431

    Google Scholar 

  • Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, Burgund ED, Grimes AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2006) A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50:799–812

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Drost UC, Rieger M, Braß M, Gunter TC, Prinz W (2005) Action-effect coupling in pianists. Psychol Res 69:233–241

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner B, Hommel B (2001) Effect anticipation and action control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:229–240

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner K, Kunde W, Kiesel A (2006) Limited transfer of subliminal response priming to novel stimulus appearances and identities (Manuscript submitted)

  • Exner S (1879) Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde. In: Herrmann L (ed) Handbuch der Physiologie, 2. Band, 2. Theil, Leipzig Vogel, pp 189–350

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG (1970) Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychol Rev 77(2):73–99

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer PM (1999) Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. Am Psychol 54:493–503

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harleß E (1861) Der apparat des willens. Z Philos philos Krit 38:50–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (2001) Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends Cogn Sci 5:253–261

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann J (1993) Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Hogrefe, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann J (2003) Anticipatory behavioral control. In: Butz M, Sigaud O, Gerard P (eds) Anticipatory behavior in adaptive learning systems. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 44–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel B (1996) The cognitive representation of action: automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychol Res 59:176–186

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel B (2000) The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In: Monsell S, Driver L (eds) Control of cognitve processes: attention and performance XVIII. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 247–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–878

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890/1981) The principles of psychology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (original work published 1890)

  • Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2004) Variable action effects: response control by contex-specific effect anticipations. Psychol Res 68:155–162

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel A, Kunde W, Pohl C, Hoffmann J (2006) Priming from novel masked stimuli depends on target set size. Adv Cogn Psychol 2:37–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel A, Kunde W, Hoffmann J (2007) Unconscious priming according to multiple S-R rules. Cognition (in press)

  • Klauer KC, Eder A, Greenwald AG, Abrams RL (2006). Priming of semantic classification by novel subliminal prime words. Conscious Cogn 16(1):63–83

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch I, Kunde W (2002) Verbal response-effect compatibility. Mem Cogn 30:1297–1303

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:387–394

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W (2003) Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psychol Res 67:153–159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W (2004) Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects. Psychol Res 68:91–96

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Weigelt M (2005) Goal congruency in bimanual object manipulation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:145–156

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Hoffmann J, Zellmann P (2002) The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning. Acta Psychol 109:137–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2003) Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition 88(2):223–242

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Koch I, Hoffmann J (2004) Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. Q J Exp Psychol A 57:87–106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2005) On the masking and disclosure of unconscious elaborate processing. A reply to van Opstal, Reynvoet, and Verguts. Cognition 97:99–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde W, Müsseler J, Heuer H (2007) Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Hum Factors (in press)

  • Lotze H (1852) Medicinische psychologie oder physiologie der seele. E. J. Bonset, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod C (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109:163–203

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:75–78

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Merikle PM, Joorden s (1997) Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without awareness. Conscious Cogn 6:219–236

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell S (1996) Control of mental processes. In: Bruce V (ed) Unresolved mysteries of the mind. Erlbaum, Hove, pp 93–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell S (2003) Task switching. Trends Cogn Sci 7:134–140

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser U (1967) Cognitive Psychology. Prentice-Hall, Englewood

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann O, Klotz W (1994) Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In: Umilta C, Moscovitch M (eds) Attention and performance XV. Conscious and nonconscious information processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 123–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Paelecke M, Kunde W (2007) Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the PRP paradigm. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform (in press)

  • Prinz W (1987) Ideomotor action. In: Heuer H, Sanders AF (eds) Perspectives on perception and action. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 47–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz W (1997) Perception and action planning. Eur J Cogn Psychol 9:129–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz W (1998) Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung. Psychol Rundsch 49:10–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders A (1980) Stage analysis of reaction processes. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp 331–354

  • Sanders AF (1998) Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and attention in human skill. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg S (1969) The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychol 30:276–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stürmer B, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2000) Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1746–1759

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stock A, Stock C (2004) A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychol Res 68:176–188

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford University press, New York

  • Rieger M (2004) Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:555–565

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Opstal F, Reynvoet B, Verguts T (2005) Unconscious semantic categorization and mask interactions: an elaborate response to Kunde et al. (2005). Cognition 97:107–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Gharamani Z, Flanagan JR (2002) Perspectives and problems in motor learning. Trends Cogn Sci 5:487–494

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Michael Ziessler for the helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. This work was supported by a grant of the German Research Foundation (DFG) KU 1964.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wilfried Kunde.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kunde, W., Elsner, K. & Kiesel, A. No anticipation–no action: the role of anticipation in action and perception. Cogn Process 8, 71–78 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2

Keywords

Navigation