Skip to main content
Log in

Gestalt compositionality and instruction-based meaning construction

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abtract

We would like to propose a new model of meaning construction based on language comprehension considered as a dynamic process during which the meaning of each linguistic unit and the global meaning of the sentence are determined simultaneously. This model, which may be called “gestalt compositionality,” is radically opposed to the classic compositional mechanism advocated by linguistic formalism based on the primacy of syntax. The process considers the syntactic structure of an utterance as the product of meaning construction rather than its source. The comprehension of an utterance is consequently directly based on the interaction between the different basic components of this utterance: lexical units, grammatical markers, positional relations between units, and more generally, basic “constructions” in the sense of Construction Grammar. Thus, meaning is really the result of a gestalt compositional process insomuch as the contribution of each basic component depends on the contribution of the other components present in the utterance. We show a first attempt at modeling from French and English examples.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Notes

  1. “Representation” is understood not in the sense usually encountered in cognitive science, but in its original latin etymom: repraesentareto make present.

  2. “Find”, “room,” and “background” may not actually be that polysemous in English.

  3. A detailed discussion that notably takes into account solutions based on mechanisms of recategorization is to be found in Fuchs et al. (1991).

  4. We agree on this point with Benveniste (1966: 123–124): “a sentence is a whole which is not reducible to the sum of its parts; the meaning inherent in this whole is distributed over the entire components. A word is a component of a sentence, it is part of its meaning but it is not present in the sentence with the meaning it has as an independent unit.”

  5. The term “instructional grammar” used here is different from the one used in language teaching by Delbart (2007) for whom the structural mechanism of a language give instructions leading to binary answers in “yes” or “no”.

  6. The distinction between “entity” and “process” is close to Langacker’s (1987) between “things” and “process,” and it encompasses a lack of temporal profile for the former and a non-zero temporal profile for the latter.

  7. See the following part for detailed analysis of examples in French and in English.

  8. “When we talk about the notion we are talking about a set that we can express, for example by ‘lire; lecture; livre; lecteur; bibliothèque; etc.’ [‘read, reading, book, reader, library etc.’], which is to say that things cannot be reduced to a lexical unit: the unit merely serves as an introduction, a first step […]. The problem is that the notion is something virtual and productive. It is not given once and for all in each meaning and that is why it cannot correspond to a lexical unit. It is a generator of lexical units; it defines a class of linked operations. It designates.” (Culioli 1990: 53–54).

  9. See further down this paragraph details about the notion of pinpointing and more generally about the role of metalinguistic operations.

  10. The capital letter indicates an entity and not a “notion of entity,” and “n” indicates that this entity comes from a notion.

  11. Up to here, our work has focused on simple utterances, in order to set up our methodology and formalization. We have nevertheless tackled the topic of subordination through the question of focalization in Col (2011), without going deeper into the question.

  12. What interrupts the unfolding of the scene here is the combination of a WHEN time clause, whose verb in the preterit tense describes the completion of a process, and of a main clause whose process is described imperfectively. With such an utterance, we actually cannot decide wether the train left the station or not, as the speaker only shows the beginning of the unfolding of the first process on the scene.

  13. See Culioli (1995: 34): “[notions] are complex representational systems of physico-cultural properties, that is to say, properties of objects resulting from manipulations necessarily a part of cultures, and from this point of view, examining notions inevitably implies speaking of problems of the province of disciplines that cannot be reduced solely to linguistics.”

  14. As we shall see in the present perfect analysis, the global analysis for the «ai vu» sequence could be divided up into more steps.

  15. The exact nature of this extraction operation that consists in building an occurrence from a notion explains why the most common use of un is to introduce a new entity, even if the generic uses are not excluded, as in Un Alsacien, ça mange de la choucroute (an alsacian (=any alsacian) simply will eat choucroute).

  16. Since an entity E1 corresponding to the speaker is already present on the scene.

  17. We have chosen to analyze a rather simple example with no complex nominal syntagm. The presence of an adjective in a nominal clause implies combining the instruction provided by the noun and the instruction provided by the adjective, the definition of which depends on the kind of adjective, its position, its reach, etc. This work is very complex and has still to be done.

  18. We ought to say that such a stabilization is quite relative, since the meaning of the words pièce and tableau can vary under different enunciation conditions. A context in which the word pièce referring to drama is totally possible, with tableau keeping its several possible acceptations. In such a case, dans would get a temporal meaning, which is allowed by its schematic form.

  19. See Col (2001) for a detailed presentation of the schematic form of already, from a topological point of view and in relation to the construction of the verbal scene.

References

  • Aptekman J, Col G (2010) Construction du sens par instructions dynamiques. Rencontres interdisciplinaires sur les systèmes complexes naturels et artificiels: Le Contexte. (to appear in the proceedings of the meeting)

  • Barsalou WL (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou WL (2008) Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59:617–645

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou WL (2010) Grounded cognition: past, present, and future. Top Cogn Sci 2:716–724

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beer RD (2000) Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4(3):91–99

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benveniste E (1966) Problèmes de linguistique générale. I Gallimard, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee J, Slobin D (1982) Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58(2):265–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cadiot P, Visetti YM (2001) Pour une théorie des formes sémantiques; motifs, profils, thèmes. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Col G (2001) Already from a topological point of view. Anglophonia 9:71–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Col G (2008a) Rôles de until et de by dans la mise en scène verbale. In: Paillard M (ed) Préfixation, préposition, postposition. Etude de cas. Presses Universitaires de Rennes, Rennes, pp 145–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Col G (2008b) Rôles des unités grammaticales dans la mise en scène verbale. In: Chuquet J (ed) Langage et Cognition. Presses Universitaires de Rennes, Rennes. To appear

  • Col G (2009) Activation et saillance d’unités grammaticales. Quelle place pour l’intonation dans la construction dynamique du sens? Travaux linguistiques du CerLiCO 22:59–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Col G (2010) Remarques sur le rôle de l’intonation dans le traitement de l’information. CORELA. http://edel.univ-poitiers.fr/corela/document.php?id=2323

  • Col G (2011) Focalisation, saillance et instruction de construction du sens. In: Cappeau P, Hanote S (eds) La Focalisation. Presses Universitaires de Rennes, Rennes. To appear

  • Col G (forthcoming) Eléments de grammaire instructionnelle

  • Coulson S (2001) Semantic leaps. Frame shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Culioli A (1990) Pour une linguistique de l’énonciation, 1. Ophrys, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Culioli A (1995) Cognition and representation in linguistic theory. John Benjamins Publishing, Amsterdam

  • Culioli A (1999) Pour une linguistique de l’énonciation, 3. Ophrys, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Delbart AR (2007) Jalons pour une grammaire instructionnelle de l’article français. In: Bouchard D, Evrard I, Vocaj E (eds) Représentation du sens linguistique. De Boeck, Bruxelles, pp 91–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Ducrot O (1984) Le Dire et le dit. Editions de Minuit, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier G (1984) Espaces mentaux, Aspects de la construction du sens dans les langues naturelles. Editions de Minuit, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier G (1991) Subdivision cognitive. Communications 53:229–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier G (1997) Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier G, Turner M (1996) Blending as a central process of grammar. In: Goldberg A (ed) Conceptual structure, discourse, and language. CSLI Publications, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Franckel JJ, Lebaud D (1990) Les Figures du sujet. Ophrys, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs C, Gosselin L, Victorri B (1991) Polysémie, glissements de sens et calcul des types de procès. Travaux de Linguistique et de Philologie

  • Goldberg A (1995) Constructions. A construction grammar approach to argument structure. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosselin L (1996) Sémantique de la temporalité en français–Un modèle calculatoire et cognitif du temps et de l’aspect. Duculot, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosselin L (2005) Temporalité et modalité. Duculot, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosselin L (2010) Les modalités en français. Rodopi

  • Guillaume P [1919] (1979) La Psychologie de la forme. Flammarion, Paris

  • Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg J (1970) Attention, organisation and consciousness. In: Mostofsky DI (ed) Attention: contemporary theory and analysis. Apelton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp 99–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird P (1983) Mental models. University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch W (2001) Predication. Cogn Sci 25:173–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1929) Gestalt psychology. Liveright, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacheret-Dujour A (2003) La Prosodie des circonstants. Peeters, Louvain

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacheret-Dujour A, Victorri B (2002) La période intonative comme unité d’analyse pour l’étude du français parlé: modélisation prosodique et enjeux linguistiques. Verbum, 55–72

  • Lacheret-Dujour A, Ploux S, Victorri B (1998) Prosodie et thématisation en français parlé. Cahiers de praxématique 30:89–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker R (1987) Foundation of cognitive grammar, 1. Stanford University Press, Stanford

  • Le Ny JF (1992) Cognitive science and semantic representations. In: Ezquerro J, Larrazabal JM (eds) Cognition, semantics and philosophy. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 273–292

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Ny JF (2004) Eléments de psycholinguistique cognitive: des représentations à la cognition. In: Fuchs C (ed) La Linguistique cognitive. Ophrys, Paris, pp 155–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Ny JF (2005) Comment l’esprit produit du sens. Odile Jacob, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara TP (2005) Semantic priming: perspectives from memory and word recognition. Psychology Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty M (1954) Phénoménologie de la perception. Gallimard, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Nygaard LC, Herold DS, Namy LL (2009) The semantics of prosody: acoustic and perceptual evidence of prosodic correlates to word meaning. Cogn Sci 33:127–146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partee BH (1997) The development of formal semantics in linguistic theory. In: Lappin S (ed) The handbook of contemporary semantic theory. Blackwell, London, pp 11–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal V, Visetti YM (1999) Sens et Temps de la Gestalt. Intellectica 28(1):147–227

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruhl C (1989) On monosemy: a study in linguistic semantics. University of New York Press, New York

  • Smith LB, Thelen E (2003) Development as a dynamic system. Trends Cogn Sci 7:343–348

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spivey M (2007) The continuity of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Talmy L (2000) Towards a cognitive semantics. MIT Press, Cambridge

  • Van der Gucht F, Willems K, De Cuypere L (2007) The iconicity of embodied meaning. Polysemy of spatial prepositions in the cognitive framework. Lang Sci 29:733–754

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gelder T (1998) The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 21:615–665

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Victorri B (1998) La construction dynamique du sens: un défi pour l’Intelligence Artificielle, Proceedings of RFIA’98 2:5–29

  • Victorri B (1999) Le sens grammatical. Langages 136:85–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Victorri B (2003) Langage et géométrie: l’expression langagière des relations spatiales. Revue de Synthèse 124:119–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Victorri B, Fuchs C (1996) La Polysémie. Hermès, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Visetti YM, Cadiot P (2002) Instability and the theory of semantic forms. In: Kurzon SD, Feigenbaum S (eds) Prepositions in their syntactic, semantic and pragmatic context. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gilles Col.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Col, G., Aptekman, J., Girault, S. et al. Gestalt compositionality and instruction-based meaning construction. Cogn Process 13, 151–170 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-011-0431-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-011-0431-y

Keywords

Navigation