Skip to main content
Log in

On the origins of endogenous thoughts

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Endogenous thoughts are thoughts that we activate in a top-down manner or in the absence of the appropriate stimuli. We use endogenous thoughts to plan or recall past events. In this sense, endogenous thinking is one of the hallmarks of our cognitive lives. In this paper, I investigate how it is that we come to possess endogenous control over our thoughts. Starting from the close relation between language and thinking, I look into speech production—a process motorically controlled by the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Interestingly, IFG is also closely related to silent talking, as well as volition. The connection between IFG and volition is important given that endogenous thoughts are or at least greatly resemble voluntary actions. Against this background, I argue that IFG is key to understanding the origins of conscious endogenous thoughts. Furthermore, I look into goal-directed thinking and show that IFG plays a key role also in unconscious endogenous thinking.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See (Tillas 2011, 2016) for a discussion of how we form concepts that do not pick out any tangible entities like democracy and justice.

  2. The notion of sub-activation refers not to the equivalents of discrete values ‘0’–neuron not firing–and ‘1’–neuron firing – but to values within the range 0–1 (see also Trafford and Tillas 2015; for a detailed discussion of related issues).

  3. The present view also differs from semantic nets as it builds upon personal experience through which the subject acquires information about the world, while semantic memory nets do not focus on personal experience.

  4. These connections weightings have been later on used to account for typicality effects (Collins and Loftus 1975).

  5. Similar issues arise for homophones to the extent that they have to be disambiguated, pronouns whose referents have to be fixed, and so forth. The points made in the paper apply also to these cases.

  6. It is worth clarifying that speech production is also informed by parts of the auditory cortex. For instance, Behroozmand et al. (2015) show that speaking is underlain by activation of a complex network of brain regions including bilateral superior temporal gyrus (STG), Heschl's gyrus, precentral gyrus, supplementary motor area (SMA), Rolandic operculum, post-central gyrus and right IFG. Furthermore, Behroozmand et al. show that for vowel sound production, the areas of Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) activation increase during speech production also include bilateral precentral gyrus, SMA, IFG, post-central gyrus and insula.

  7. Intrusive thoughts are most often construed as repeatedly occurring thoughts that might be unwanted or negative. Specifically, subjects were asked to rate the extent to which statements such as ‘Today, I cannot get certain thoughts out of my mind’ and ‘Today, I keep thinking about something over and over again’, as well as a statement concerning self-related intrusive thoughts, ‘Today, I have difficulties suppressing thoughts about myself’, match their thoughts as well as their feeling during a given day, on a scale (0–does not apply at all to 7–does apply very well. The average score for all three items for the one hundred days of the study indicated the average amount and intensity of intrusive thoughts during the time of the study.

  8. Regional Homogeneity is a voxel-based measure of brain activity, which evaluates the similarity or synchronisation between the time series of a given voxel and its nearest neighbours (Zang et al. 2004).

  9. I use ‘computationally’ here in the broad sense of the term.

  10. The transition between different thoughts following ‘I want food’ such ‘Shall I go for x or y?’ are easily understood in terms of associations.

References

  • Ackermann H (2008) Cerebellar contributions to speech production and speech perception: psycholinguistic and neurobiological perspectives. Trends Neurosci 31(6):265–272. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.011

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA (1990) Goal ≠ Intent: Goal-Directed Thought and Behavior Are Often Unintentional. Psychol Inq 1(3):248–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–609

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baym CL, Corbett BA, Wright SB, Bunge SA (2008) Neural correlates of tic severity and cognitive control in children with Tourette syndrome. Brain J Neurol 131:165–179

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Behroozmand R, Shebek R, Hansen DR, Oya H, Robin DA, Howard MA, Greenlee JD (2015) Sensory-motor networks involved in speech production and motor control: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 109:418–428. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.040

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berk L (1994) Why children talk to themselves. Sci Am 271(5):78–83

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berk L, Garvin R (1984) Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Dev Psychol 20(2):271–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bivens JA, Berk LE (1990) A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children’s private speech. Merrill Palmer Q 36:443–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth JR, Wood L, Lu D, Houk JC, Bitan T (2007) The role of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in language processing. Brain Res 1133(1):136–144. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.074

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandom R (1994) Making it explicit: reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Buse J, August J, Bock N, Dorfel D, Rothenberger A, Roessner V (2012) Fine motor skills and interhemispheric transfer in treatment–naïve male children with Tourette syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol 54:629–635

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (1998) Conscious thinking: language or elimination? Mind Lang 13(4):457–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (2005) Consciousness: essays from a higher order perspective. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (2008) Language in cognition. In: Margolis E, Samuels R, Stich S (eds) The Oxford handbook of philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 382–401

    Google Scholar 

  • Christoff K, Ream JM, Gabrieli JD (2004) Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 40(4–5):623–630

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (1998) Magic words: how language augments human computation. In: Carruthers P, Boucher J (eds) Language and thought: interdisciplinary themes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 162–183

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A, Chalmers DJ (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):7–19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins AM, Quillian MR (1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 8(2):240–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins AM, Loftus EF (1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol Rev 82(6):407–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1989) Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 33(1–2):25–62

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson D (1975) Thought and talk. In: Davidson D (ed) Inquiries into truth and interpretation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 155–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness explained. Little Brown, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Dronkers NF (1996) A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384:159–161

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elman JL, Bates EA, Johnson MH, Karmiloff-Smith A, Parisi D, Plunkett K (1996) Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on development. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Filevich E, Kuhn S, Haggard P (2012) Intentional inhibition in human action: the power of ‘no’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1107–1118

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Filimon F, Nelson JD, Hagler DJ, Sereno MI (2007) Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery. Neuroimage 37(4):1315–1328

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Findlay JM, Gilchrist ID (2003) Active vision: the psychology of looking and seeing. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1978) Representations: philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1983) The modularity of mind: an essay in faculty psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1987) Psychosemantics: the problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganos C, Kahl U, Brandt V, Schunke O, Bäumer T, Thomalla G, Roessner V, Haggard P, Münchau A, Kühn S (2014) The neural correlates of tic inhibition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychologia 65:297–301

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauker C (1990) How to learn a language like a Chimpanzee. Philos Psychol 3(1):31–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlee JD, Oya H, Kawasaki H, Volkov IO, Kaufman OP, Kovach C, Howard MA, Brugge JF (2004) A functional connection between inferior frontal gyrus and orofacial motor cortex in human. J Neurophysiol 92:1153–1164

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlee JD, Oya H, Kawasaki H, Volkov IO, Severson MA, Howard MA, Brugge JF (2007) Functional connections within the human inferior frontal gyrus. J Comp Neurol 503:550–559

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1957) Meaning. Philos Rev 66:377–388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1968) Utterer’s meaning, sentence meaning, and word-meaning. Found Lang 4:225–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1969) Utterer’s meaning and intentions. Philos Rev 68:147–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1989) Studies in the ways of words. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 3(3):e79. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Indefrey P, Levelt WJ (2004) The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92(1–2):101–144. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (1996) How language helps us think. Pragmat Cogn 4:1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) The principles of psychology, vol 2. Henry Holt, New York (Reprinted Bristol: Thoemmes Press, 1999)

  • Kühn S, Schmiedek F, Brose A, Schott BH, Lindenberger U, Lövden M (2013) The neural representation of intrusive thoughts. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:688–693

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo LE, Kitlinska JB, Tilan JU et al (2007) Neuropeptide Y acts directly in the periphery on fat tissue and mediates stress-induced obesity and metabolic syndrome. Nat Med 13(7):803–811. doi:10.1038/nm1611

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leisman G, Machado C, Melillo R, Mualem R (2012) Intentionality and “free-will” from a neurodevelopmental perspective. Front Integr Neurosci 6:36

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Locke J (1690/1975) An essay concerning human understanding. Oxford University Press, New York

  • Lupyan G (2007) Reuniting categories, language and perception. In: McNamara DS, Trafton JG (eds) Twenty-ninth annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, pp 1247–1252

    Google Scholar 

  • Marieb E, Marieb E (2010) Human anatomy and physiology, 8th edn. Pearson Benjamin Cummings, San Francisco, pp 945–947

    Google Scholar 

  • Martinez C, Do V, Martinez JL, Derrick BE (2002) Associative long-term potentiation (LTP) among extrinsic afferents of the hippocampal CA3 region in vivo. Brain Res 940:86–94

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Murray RM, David AS, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (1996) Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychol Med 26(1):29–38

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morin A, Michaud J (2007) Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: inner speech use during self-related processing. Brain Res Bull 74:387–396

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan G (2012) Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Front Psychol 3:54. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Paszek J, Pollok B, Biermann-Ruben K, Muller-Vahl K, Roessner V, Thomalla G et al (2010) Is it a tic?—Twenty seconds to make a diagnosis. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 25:1106–1108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex (trans: Anrep GV). Oxford University Press, London

  • Prinz J (2002) Furnishing the mind: concepts and their perceptual basis. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz J (2011) Has Mentalese earned its keep? On Jerry Fodor’s LOT 2. Mind 120(478):485–501

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart DE, Smolensky P, McClelland JL, Hinton GE (1986) Parallel distributed models of schemata and sequential thought processes. In: McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE (eds) Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: psychological and biological models. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 7–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Shergill SS, Brammer MJ, Fukuda R et al (2002) Modulation of activity in temporal cortex during generation of inner speech. Hum Brain Mapp 16:219–227

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taheri S, Lin L, Austin D, Young T, Mignot E (2004) Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Med 1(3):e62

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tillas A (2011) Back to our senses: an empiricist on concept acquisition. Doctoral Thesis. University of Bristol, UK

  • Tillas A (2014) How do ideas become general in their signification?. Baltic international yearbook of cognition, logic and communication, vol 9, issue 1. Kansas State University

  • Tillas A (2015) Language as grist to the mill of cognition. Cogn Process 16(3):219–243

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillas A (2016) On the origins of concepts. In: Kann C, Hommen D, Osswald T (eds) Concepts and categorization. Mentis, Paderborn

    Google Scholar 

  • Tillas A, Vosgerau V (2016) Perception, action and the notion of grounding. In: Müller VC (ed) Fundamental issues of artificial intelligence, Synthese Library, Springer

  • Trafford J, Tillas A (2015) Intuition and reason: re-assessing dual-process theories with representational sub-activation. Teorema 34(3):197–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Tullett AM, Inzlicht M (2010) The voice of self-control: blocking the inner voice increases impulsive responding. Acta Psychol 135(2):252–256. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky LS (1962) Thought and language. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1953) Philosophical investigations. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Yee E, Overton E, Thompson-Schill SL (2009) Looking for meaning: eye movements are sensitive to overlapping semantic features, not association. Psychon Bull Rev 16(5):869–874

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng ZZ, Munhall KG, Johnsrude IS (2010) Functional overlap between regions involved in speech perception and in monitoring one’s own voice during speech production. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1770–1781. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21324

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexandros Tillas.

Additional information

Handling editor: Katsumi Watanabe (University of Tokyo).

Reviewers: Alain Morin (Mount Royal University), Zane Zheng (Lasell College), Lila Chrysikou (University of Kansas).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tillas, A. On the origins of endogenous thoughts. Cogn Process 18, 107–117 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0786-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0786-1

Keywords

Navigation