Skip to main content
Log in

Assessing orienting of attention to understand the time course of mental calculation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Number processing induces spatial attention shifts to the left or right side for small or large numbers, respectively. This spatial-numerical association (SNA) extends to mental calculation, such that subtractions and additions induce left or right biases, respectively. However, the time course of activating SNAs during mental calculation is unclear. Here, we addressed this issue by measuring visual position discrimination during auditory calculation. Thirty-four healthy adults listened in each trial to five successive elements of arithmetic facts (first operand, operator, second operand, equal and result) and verbally classified their correctness. After each element (except for the result), a fixation dot moved equally often to either the left or right side and participants pressed left or right buttons to discriminate its movement direction (four times per trial). First and second operand magnitude (small/large), operation (addition/subtraction), result correctness (right/wrong) and movement direction (left/right) were balanced across 128 trials. Manual reaction times of dot movement discriminations were considered in relation to previous arithmetic elements. We found no evidence of early attentional shifts after first operand and operator presentation. Discrimination performance was modulated consistent with SNAs after the second operand, suggesting that attentional shifts occur once there is access to all elements necessary to complete an arithmetic operation. Such late-occurring attention shifts may reflect a combination of multiple element-specific biases and confirm their functional role in mental calculation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Following this design, the probability to perform a subtraction or an addition after hearing a large or a small number was the same.

References

  • Anelli F, Lugli L, Baroni G, Borghi AM, Nicoletti R (2014) Walking boosts your performance in making additions and subtractions. Front Psychol 5:1459. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01459

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh R, Dowker A (2015) The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition. University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan N (1984) On short and long auditory stores. Psychol Bull 96:341–370

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S (2011) The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122(3):371–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Naccache L, Le Clec’H G, Koechlin E, Mueller M, Dehaene-Lambertz G et al (1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395:597–600

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39:175–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer B, Breitmeyer BG (1987) Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements. Neuropsychologia 25:73–83

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH, Shaki S (2014) Spatial associations in numerical cognition-From single digits to arithmetic. Q J Exp Psychol 67:1461–1483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groen GJ, Parkman JM (1972) A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychol Rev 79(4):329–343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann M, Mast FW, Fischer MH (2015a) Counting is a spatial process: evidence from eye movements. Psychol Res 80:399–409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0722-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann M, Mast FW, Fischer MH (2015b) Spatial biases during mental arithmetic: evidence from eye movements on blank screen. Front Psychol 6:12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knops A, Viarouge A, Dehaene S (2009) Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: evidence from the operational momentum effect. Atten Percept Psychophys 71(4):803–821

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu D, Cai D, Verguts T, Chen Q (2017) The time course of spatial attention shifts in elementary arithmetic. Sci Rep 7:921. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-01037-3

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Masson N, Pesenti M (2014) Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. Q J Exp Psychol 67:1514–1526. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.903985

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masson N, Pesenti M (2016) Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation. Psychol Res 80(4):640–651

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masson N, Letesson C, Pesenti M (2018) Time course of overt attentional shifts in mental arithmetic: evidence from gaze metrics. Q J Exp Psychol 71(4):1009–1019. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1318931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathieu R, Gourjon A, Couderc A, Thevenot C, Prado J (2016) Running the number line: rapid shifts of attention in single-digit arithmetic. Cognition 146:229–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrink K, Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G (2007) Moving along the number line: operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Percept Psychophys 69:1324–1333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk H-C, Moeller K, Klein E, Willmes K, Fischer MH (2011) Extending the mental number line: a review of multi-digit number processing. J Psychol 219(1):3–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinhas M, Fischer MH (2008) Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition 109:408–415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinhas M, Shaki S, Fischer MH (2014) Heed the signs: operation signs have spatial associations. Q J Exp Psychol 67:1527–1540. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.892516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Petersen SE, Fox PT, Raichle ME (1988) Localization of cognitive operations in the brain. Science 240:1627–1631

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reynvoet B, Brysbaert M, Fias W (2002) Semantic priming in number naming. Q J Exp Psychol 55A(4):1127–1139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaki S, Fischer MH (2014) Random walks on the mental number line. Exp Brain Res 232:43–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaki S, Pinhas M, Fischer MH (2017) Heuristics and biases in mental arithmetic: revisiting and reversing operational momentum. Think Reason. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2017.1348987

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185:1124–1131

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Uittenhove K, Thevenot C, Barrouillet P (2016) Fast automated counting procedures in addition problem solving: when are they used and why are they mistaken for retrieval? Cognition 146:289–303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins OC, Watkins MJ (1980) The modality effect and echoic persistence. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:251–278

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by “Competing heuristics and biases in mental arithmetic” (Grant No. DFG FI_1915/8-1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefania D’Ascenzo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Handling editor: Juan Lupiáñez (University of Granada).

Reviewers: Pom Charras (University Paul Valery Montpellier) and a second researcher who preferred to remain anonymous.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

D’Ascenzo, S., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R. et al. Assessing orienting of attention to understand the time course of mental calculation. Cogn Process 21, 493–500 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00970-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00970-y

Keywords

Navigation