Skip to main content
Log in

The elephant in the room: attention to salient scene features increases with comedic expertise

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What differentiates the joke writing strategy employed by professional comedians from non-comedians? Previous MRI work found that professional comedians relied to a greater extent on “bottom-up processes,” i.e., associations driven by the prompt stimuli themselves, while controls relied more on prefrontal lobe directed, “top-down” processes. In the present work, professional improv comedians and controls generated humorous captions to cartoons while their eye movements were tracked. Participants’ visual fixation patterns were compared to predictions of the saliency model (Harel et al. in Adv Neural Inf Process Syst 19:545–552, 2007)—a computer model for identifying the most salient locations in an image based on visual features. Captions generated by the participants were rated for funniness by independent raters. Relative to controls, professional comedians’ gaze was driven to a greater extent by the cartoons’ salient visual features. For all participants, captions’ funniness positively correlated with visual attention to salient cartoon features. Results suggest that comedic expertise is associated with increased reliance on bottom-up, stimulus-driven creativity, and that a bottom-up strategy results, on average, in funnier captions whether employed by comedians or controls. The cognitive processes underlying successful comedic creativity appear to adhere to the old comedians’ adage “pay attention to the elephant in the room.”

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

© reprinted with permission). Middle right: saliency model predictions of eye-fixations based on low-level features. Left two columns: actual overall fixations of improv comedians and controls during the 3 experimental conditions (HUM, EXP, NOTH). Circles indicate visual fixations. Circle size indicates the total overall time spent fixating on a particular location. Circle color is meaningless, the purpose of varying the color is allowing a better understanding of the heatmaps when the fixation-circles partially overlap. Numbers under the heatmaps are ROC scores—the higher the score, the greater the fit between the model and the fixations

Fig. 2
Fig. 3

© reprinted with permission). Middle right: saliency model predictions of eye-fixations based on low-level visual features. Left two columns: actual overall fixations of improv comedians and controls during the trials that resulted in captions receiving the higher ratings (top) vs. lower ratings (bottom) all within the humorous condition. Circles indicate visual fixations. Circle size indicates the total overall time spent fixating on a particular location. Circle color is meaningless, the purpose of varying the color is allowing a better understanding of the heatmaps when the fixation-circles partially overlap. Numbers under the heatmaps are ROC scores—the higher the score, the greater the fit between the model and the fixations

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. A saliency of a feature is often defined, in such context, to indicate the degree to which the feature attracts attention in the absence of a goal-oriented task (e.g., during free viewing; see Itti et al. 1998).

  2. Anything that the joke is written about, e.g., a cartoon, improvised scene, news item.

  3. Note that a process of the inhibition of previously activated associations may occur in the absence of top-down intervention (Martindale 2007). This extends to overt attention: in eye-tracking studies in which participates engage in a task free, free viewing of images, participants often do not redirect their gaze into image locations they recently scanned—a phenomenon known as inhibition of return (Wang & Klein, 2010).

References

  • Amir O (2016) The frog test: a tool for measuring humor theories’ validity and humor preferences. Front Hum Neurosci 10:40

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Amir O, Biederman I (2016) The neural correlates of humor creativity. Front Hum Neurosci 10:597

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Amir O, Xu X, Biederman I (2011) The spontaneous appeal by naïve subjects to nonaccidental properties when distinguishing among highly similar members of subspecies of birds closely resembles descriptions produced by experts. In: Vision sciences society annual meeting

  • Amir O, Biederman I, Wang Z, Xu X (2013) Ha Ha! Versus Aha! A direct comparison of humor to nonhumorous insight for determining the neural correlates of mirth. Cereb Cortex 25(5):1405–1413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aziz-Zadeh L, Liew SL, Dandekar F (2013) Exploring the neural correlates of visual creativity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8(4):475–480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basadur M (1995) Optimal ideation-evaluation ratios. Creat Res J 8(1):63–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty RE, Silvia PJ, Nusbaum EC, Jauk E, Benedek M (2014) The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Mem Cognit 42(7):1186–1197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty RE, Kenett YN, Christensen AP, Rosenberg MD, Benedek M, Chen Q, Silvia PJ (2018) Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 115(5):1087–1092

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal J, Sánchez FJ, Vilariño F, Arnold M, Ghosh A, Lacey G (2014) Experts vs. novices: applying eye-tracking methodologies in colonoscopy video screening for polyp search. In: Proceedings of the symposium on eye tracking research and applications, pp 223–226

  • Bowden EM, Jung-Beeman M, Fleck J, Kounios J (2005) New approaches to demystifying insight. Trends Cogn Sci 9:322–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brawer J, Amir O (2021) Mapping the ‘funny bone’: neuroanatomical correlates of humor creativity in professional comedians. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci

  • Chen Q, Beaty RE, Cui Z, Sun J, He H, Zhuang K, Qiu J (2019) Brain hemispheric involvement in visuospatial and verbal divergent thinking. Neuroimage 202:116065

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen Q, Beaty RE, Qiu J (2020) Mapping the artistic brain: common and distinct neural activations associated with musical, drawing, and literary creativity. Hum Brain Map

  • Dalmaijer ES, Mathôt S, Van der Stigchel S (2014) PyGaze: an open-source, cross-platform toolbox for minimal-effort programming of eye tracking experiments. Behav Res Methods 46:913–921

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich A, Kanso R (2010) A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychol Bull 136:822–848

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erhard K, Kessler F, Neumann N, Ortheil HJ, Lotze M (2014) Professional training in creative writing is associated with enhanced fronto-striatal activity in a literary text continuation task. Neuroimage 100:15–23

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith E, Kane MJ, Welhaf MS, Christensen AP, Silvia PJ, Beaty RE (2021) Keeping creativity under control: contributions of attention control and fluid intelligence to divergent thinking. Creat Res J 1–20

  • Gegenfurtner A, Lehtinen E, Säljö R (2011) Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations: a meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains. Educ Psychol Rev 23(4):523–552

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greengross G, Martin RA, Miller G (2012) Personality traits, intelligence, humor styles, and humor production ability of professional stand-up comedians compared to college students. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts 6(1):74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harding P, Robertson NM (2009) A comparison of feature detectors with passive and task-based visual saliency. In: Scandinavian conference on image analysis, pp 716–725. Springer, Berlin

  • Harel J, Koch C, Perona P (2007) Graph-based visual saliency. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst 19:545–552

    Google Scholar 

  • Harel J, Koch C, Perona P (2006) Graph-based visual saliency. Advances in neural information processing systems, 19

  • Hirsch J, Curcio CA (1989) The spatial resolution capacity of human foveal retina. Vis Res 29(9):1095–1101

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Houston JP, Mednick SA (1963) Creativity and the need for novelty. Psychol Sci Public Interest 66(2):137

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Howard-Jones PA, Blakemore SJ, Samuel EA, Summers IR, Claxton G (2005) Semantic divergence and creative story generation: an fMRI investigation. Cogn Brain Res 25:240–250

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE, Biederman I (1992) Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. Psychol Rev 99(3):480

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley MM, Dennett DC, Adams RB Jr., Adams RB (2011) Inside jokes: using humor to reverse-engineer the mind. MIT Press

  • Itti L, Koch C (2000) A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vis Res 40(10–12):1489–1506

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Itti L (2000) Models of bottom-up and top-down visual attention. California Institute of Technology

  • Itti L, Koch C (2001) Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(3):194–203

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Itti L, Koch C, Niebur E (1998) A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell 20(11):1254–1259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itti L (2000) Models of bottom-up and top-down visual attention. California Institute of Technology

  • Kinchla RA, Wolfe JM (1979) The order of visual processing:“Top-down”,“bottom-up”, or “middle-out.” Percept Psychophys 25(3):225–231

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Le Meur O, Le Callet P, Barba D, Thoreau D (2006) A coherent computational approach to model the bottom-up visual attention. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell 28:802–817

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Meur O, Le Callet P, Barba D (2007) Predicting visual fixations on video based on low-level visual features. Vis Res 47(19):2483–2498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limb CJ, Braun AR (2008) Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: an fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE 3:e1679. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001679

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw AP, Warren C (2010) Benign violations: Making immoral behavior funny. Psychol Sci 21(8):1141–1149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin RA, Ford T (2018) The psychology of humor: an integrative approach. Academic press

  • Martindale C (2007) Creativity, primordial cognition, and personality. Personality Individ Differ 43(7):1777–1785. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick S (1962) The associative basis of the creative process. Psychol Rev 69(3):220. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048850

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melloni L, van Leeuwen S, Alink A, Müller NG (2012) Interaction between bottom-up saliency and top-down control: how saliency maps are created in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 22(12):2943–2952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon D, Kasten R (2011) A demonstration of direct access to colored stimuli following cueing by color. Acta Physiol (oxf) 138(1):30–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkhurst D, Law K, Niebur E (2002) Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vis Res 42(1):107–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinho AL, de Manzano O, Fransson P, Eriksson H, Ullen F (2014) Connecting to create: expertise in musical improvisation is associated with increased functional connectivity between premotor and prefrontal areas. J Neurosci 34(18):6156–6163

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Raskin V (2012) Semantic mechanisms of humor, Vol 24. Springer

  • Rominger C, Papousek I, Perchtold CM, Benedek M, Weiss EM, Weber B, Fink A (2020) Functional coupling of brain networks during creative idea generation and elaboration in the figural domain. NeuroImage, 207

  • Ruch W (2001) The perception of humor. In: Emotions, qualia, and consciousness, pp 410–425

  • Ruch W, Attardo S, Raskin V (1993) Toward an empirical verification of the general theory of verbal humor. Humor Int J Humor Res 6(2):123–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlegel A, Alexander P, Fogelson SV, Li X, Lu Z, Kohler PJ et al (2015) The artist emerges: visual art learning alters neural structure and function. Neuroimage 105:440–451

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah C, Erhard K, Ortheil HJ, Kaza E, Kessler C, Lotze M (2013) Neural correlates of creative writing: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 34(5):1088–1101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shahaf D, Horvitz E, Mankoff R (2015) Inside jokes: identifying humorous cartoon captions. In: Proceedings of the 21th ACM SIGKDD international conference on knowledge discovery and data mining, pp 1065–1074

  • Suls JM (1972) A two-stage model for the appreciation of jokes and cartoons: an information-processing analysis. Psychol Humor Theoret Perspect Empir Issues 1:81–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Tatler BW, Baddeley RJ, Gilchrist ID (2005) Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time. Vis Res 45(5):643–659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes J (2010) Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Physiol (oxf) 135(2):77–99

    Google Scholar 

  • Villarreal MF, Cerquetti D, Caruso S, Schwarcz Lopez Aranguren V, Gerschcovich ER, Frega AL, Leiguarda RC (2013) Neural correlates of musical creativity: differences between high and low creative subjects. PLoS ONE 8(9):75427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS, Van Wert MJ, Kenner NM, Place SS, Kibbi N (2007) Low target prevalence is a stubborn source of errors in visual search tasks. J Experim Psychol :General 136(4):623

  • Wang Z, Klein RM (2010) Searching for inhibition of return in visual search: a review. Vis Res 50(2):220–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The research was supported by an NSF grant IIS-1948517 to AP and a Pomona College Faculty Grant to OA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ori Amir.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Handling Editor: Cees van Leeuwen (KU Leuven); Reviewers: Andrey Nikolaev (Lund University).

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (PDF 249 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Amir, O., Utterback, K.J., Lee, J. et al. The elephant in the room: attention to salient scene features increases with comedic expertise. Cogn Process 23, 203–215 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-022-01079-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-022-01079-0

Keywords

Navigation