Skip to main content
Log in

Reconfiguring Evidence: Interacting with Digital Objects in Scientific Practice

  • Published:
Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW) Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper analyzes how scientists working in a multidisciplinary team produce scientific evidence through building and manipulating scientific visualizations. The research is based on ethnographic observations of scientists’ weekly work meetings and the observation of videotapes of these meetings. The scientists observed work with advanced imaging technologies to produce a 4D computer model of heat transfer in human prostate tissues. The idea of ‘digital objects’ is proposed in order to conceptually locate their ‘materiality’, observed in the practices of producing evidence through the handling of three-dimensional renderings of data. The manipulation of digital objects seeks to establish meaningful differences between parameters of interest, both when building and when analyzing them. These digital objects are dealt with as part of the empirical evidence used in the course of practices of visualizing and modeling natural phenomena. This process, which can be contextualized historically in terms of the development of imaging technologies, becomes crucial in understanding what counts as empirical evidence in current scientific work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term embodied used here seeks to offer a description of the way scientists engage with the digital objects in question that is more relational than a mere passive looking at images. In this sense, I use the term “object” in place of “image”, as a means to elicit this relationship, through which scientific evidence is produced and analyzed.

  2. The paper will not engage in a discussion about the ontological status of ‘natural’ versus ‘digital’ objects, as this is not the focus of this research. Also, I believe that this question is not answerable through the data collected and analyzed herein. In terms of the premises used in the discussion that follows, it is worth mentioning, however, that I remain agnostic about the ontological materiality of either natural or digital objects analyzed. The paper will address these forms of existence as they pertain to the practices observed, and will not explore whether the digital objects are actually empirical or not. I believe this sort of symmetrical approach (Latour 1993; Latour and Woolgar 1986), geared to the scientists’ practice (Bourdieu 1997; Sterne 2003), is a more adequate stance, given the ethnographic nature of the methods used to gather my own data. This does not, however, invalidate such philosophical discussions by other authors.

  3. The natural vs. digital object dichotomy used here relates to Latour’s (1990) discussion of immutable mobiles and how they construe a relationship between natural and representational realms; it also refers to the scientists’ perceptions as observed in fieldwork. Natural thus refers to what is present in nature, the phenomena they seek to model; and digital here means that which pertains to the processed images and digital objects constructed by the scientists, which reference the natural objects. To the scientists observed, the digital objects are the closest possible renderings of natural phenomena they can possibly achieve, given the technologies and methods used. Their value as knowledge and evidence is dependent on how close they are thought to resemble the natural. This dichotomy is basic to the practices observed throughout this research: the idea of a ‘match’ between natural and digital rendering is what is sought through the various modeling and visualization practices.

  4. This ethnographic study was approved by the University of Texas at Austin’s Institutional Review Board, having met all requirements for the ethical study of human subjects (UT IRB Study No. 2006-11-0040). All names have been substituted for by pseudonyms to protect the privacy of the scientists.

  5. A specialist in scientific visualization, during a workshop I participated in, offered by the advanced computing facility available on the campus where the team I observed was located.

  6. Primarily members of the university’s research community, with some visitors from other institutions and from overseas. I was the only one from a humanities or social science background during this particular week.

  7. Transcript conventions:

    (.) a gap of approximately one tenth of a second;

    . full stop, stopping fall in tone, not necessarily end of sentence;

    (?) unintelligible;

    (word) dubious hearing

    (()) transcriber’s descriptions rather than or in addition to transcriptions.

References

  • Alač, M. (2008). Working with brain scans: digital images and gestural interaction in fMRI laboratory. Social Studies of Science, 38(4), 483–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaulieu, A. (2001). Voxels in the brain: neuroscience, informatics and changing notions of objectivity. Social Studies of Science, 31(5), 635–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaulieu, A. (2002a). A space for measuring mind and brain: interdisciplinarity and digital tools in the development of brain mapping and functional imaging, 1980–1990. Brain and Cognition, 49, 13–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaulieu, A. (2002b). Images are not the (only) truth: brain mapping, visual knowledge and iconoclasm. Science, Technology & Human Values, 27(1), 53–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becvar, L. A., Hollan, J., & Hutchins, E. (2005). Hands as molecules: representational gestures used for developing theory in a scientific laboratory. Semiotica, 156(4), 89–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bement, A., Jr. (2007). Cyberinfrastructure visions for 21st century discovery. Arlington: National Science Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1997). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chadarevian, S. D., & Hopwood, N. (2004). Models: the third dimension of science. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, J., & Kiesler, S. (2005). Collaborative research across disciplinary and organizational boundaries. Social Studies of Science, 35(5), 703–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Galison, P. (1990). The image of objectivity. Representations, 40, 81–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Galiston, P. (1990). The image of objectivity. Representations, 40, 81–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Hey, T., & Trefethen, A. (2004). UK E-science programme: next generation grid applications. International Journal of High Performance Computing Applications, 18(3), 285–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopwood, N., & Chadarevian, S. D. (2004). Dimensions of modeling. In models: the third dimension of science. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr-Cetina, K., & Amann, K. (1990). Image dissection in natural scientific inquiry. Science, Technology and Human Values, 15(3), 259–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1990). Drawing things together. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 19–68). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993). We have never been modern. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1995). The ‘Pedofil’ of Boa Vista: a photo-philosophical montage. Common Knowledge, 4(1), 144–187.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1990). The externalized retina: Selection and mathematization in the visual documentation of objects in the life sciences. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 153–186). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (2006). The production of scientific images: Vision and re-vision in the history, philosophy and sociology of science. In L. Pauwels (Ed.), Visual cultures of science: Rethinking representational practices in knowledge building and science communication (pp. 26–41). Hanover: Dartmouth College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M., & Woolgar, S. (1990a). Introduction: Sociological orientations to representational practice in science. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 1–19). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M., & Woolgar, S. (1990b). Representation in scientific practice. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteiro, M. (2010). Beyond the merely visual: interacting with digital objects in interdisciplinary scientific practice. Semiotica.

  • Monteiro, M., & Keating, E. (2009). Managing misunderstandings: the role of language in interdisciplinary scientific collaboration. Science Communication, 31(1), 6–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, N. (2008). Molecular embodiments and the body-work of modeling in protein crystallography. Social Studies of Science, 38(2), 163–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Jacoby, S., & Gonzales, P. (1994). Interpretive journeys: how physicists talk and travel through graphic space. Configurations, 2(1), 151–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Jacoby, S., & Gonzales, P. (1996). “When I come down I’m in the domain state”: grammar and graphic representation in the interpretive activity of physics. In E. Ochs, E. Schlegloff, & S. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 328–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pauwels, L. (2006a). A theoretical framework for assessing visual representational practices in knowledge building and science communications. In L. Pauwels (Ed.), Visual cultures of science: Rethinking representational practices in knowledge building and science communication (pp. 1–26). Hanover: Dartmouth College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pauwels, L. (Ed.). (2006b). Visual cultures of science: Rethinking representational practices in knowledge building and science communication. Hanover: Dartmouth College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, R. (2008). e-Sciences as research technologies: reconfiguring disciplines, globalizing knowledge. Social Science Information, 47(2), 131–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapin, S., & Schaffer, S. (1985). Leviathan and the air-pump: Hobbes, Boyle and the experimental life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterne, J. (2003). Bourdieu, technique and technology. Cultural Studies, 17(3/4), 367–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolgar, S., & Coopmans, C. (2006). Virtual witnessing in a virtual age: A prospectus for social studies of e-science. In C. Hine (Ed.), New infrastructures for knowledge production: Understanding e-science (pp. 1–26). Hershey: Information Science Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I want to thank the Science, Technology and Society Program at the University of Texas at Austin for funding the research presented in this paper. I also wanted to thank Prof. Elizabeth Keating, for making the ethnography that provided the data analyzed here possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marko Monteiro.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Monteiro, M. Reconfiguring Evidence: Interacting with Digital Objects in Scientific Practice. Comput Supported Coop Work 19, 335–354 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-010-9115-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-010-9115-x

Key words

Navigation