Skip to main content
Log in

Do great minds appear in batches?

  • Published:
Scientometrics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite much scholarly fascination with the question of whether great minds appear in cycles, together with some empirical evidence that historical cycles exist, prior studies mostly disregard the “great minds” hypothesis as it relates to scientists. Rather, researchers assume a linear relation based on the argument that science is allied with the development of technology. To probe this issue further, this study uses a ranking of over 5600 scientists based on number of appearances in Google Books over a period of 200 years (1800–2000). The results point to several peak periods, particularly for scientists born in the 1850–1859, 1897–1906, or 1900–1909 periods, suggesting overall cycles of around 8 years and a positive trend in distinction that lasts around 100 years. Nevertheless, a non-parametric test to determine whether randomness can be rejected indicates that non-randomness is less apparent, although once we analyse the greatest minds overall, rejection is more likely.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For an overview of the studies related to society and creativity, see Gowan and Olson (1979).

  2. See also Kroeber (1944) and Sorokin (1957), as well as Turchin’s examinations of historical cycles (Turchin 2003; Turchin and Nefedov 2009).

  3. One analysis of a top economics journal demonstrates that the citation peak of articles is reached after 5 years (Johnston et al. 2013). McDowell (1982), in a discussion of the rate of knowledge obsolescence or depreciation reflected in the age profile of cited works in different disciplines, finds the largest decay rate in physics and chemistry (an annual average of 18.30), followed by economics (13.18), sociology and psychology (10.82), biology (8.68), history (3.85), and English (2.67).

  4. Data available from http://fame.gonzolabs.org/datasets (Adrian Veres is a co-contributor to the data set).

  5. See http://fame.gonzolabs.org/release-notes for the details on scientist selection and construction of the mD measure.

  6. Five point 3 % of scientists are classified into two or more fields.

  7. For example, the 10-year moving average mD at time t equals t −5*1/512 + t −4*9/512 + ··· + t*126/512 + ··· + t +3*9/512 + t +4*1/512.

References

  • Aiden, E., & Michel, J.-B. (2013). Big data as a lens on human vulture. New York: Riverhead Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, M. (1995). Nineteenth-century foundations of creativity research. Creativity Research Journal, 8(3), 219–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohannon, J. (2011). The science hall of fame. Science, 331(6014), 143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gowan, J. C., & Olson, M. (1979). The society which maximizes creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 13(3), 194–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, C. E. (1966). A measurement of creativity in western civilization. American Anthropologist, 68(6), 1384–1417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. J. A. (1999). Genius explained. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1880). Great men, great thoughts and the environment. Atlantic Monthly, 46(276), 441–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, D. W., Piatti, M., & Torgler, B. (2013). Citation success over time: Theory or empirics? Scientometrics, 95(3), 1023–1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A. L. (1944). Configurations of culture growth. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, J. M. (1982). Obsolescence of knowledge and career publication profiles: Some evidence of differences among fields in costs of interrupted careers. American Economic Review, 72(4), 752–768.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R. K. (1973). The sociology of science: Theoretical and empirical investigations. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., The Google Books Team, et al. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331(6014), 176–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naroll, R., Benjamin, E. C., Fohl, F. K., Fried, M. J., Hildreth, R. E., & Schaefer, J. M. (1971). Creativity: A cross-historical pilot survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 2(2), 181–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schumpeter, J. A. (1934/1983). The theory of economic development: An inquiry into profits, capital, credit, interest, and the business cycle. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

  • Schweber, S. S. (2003). Quantum field theory: From QED to the standard model. In M. J. Nye (Ed.), The Cambridge History of Science. Volume 5. The modern physical and mathematical sciences (pp. 375–393). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweber, S. S. (2008). Einstein and oppenheimer: The meaning of genius. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1975a). Sociocultural context of individual creativity: A transhistorical time-series analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(6), 1119–1133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1975b). Age and literary creativity: A cross-cultural and transhistorical survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6(3), 259–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1976a). Philosophical eminence, beliefs, and zeitgeist: An individual-generational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(4), 630–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1976b). The Causal relation between war and scientific discovery: An exploratory cross-national analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7(2), 133–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1976c). Biographical determinants of achieved eminence: A multivariate approach to the Cox data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(2), 218–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1978). The eminent genius in history: The critical role of creative development. Gifted Child Quarterly, 22(2), 187–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1980). Thematic fame, melodic originality, and musical zeitgeist: A biographical and transhistorical content analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(6), 972–983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1984). Artistic creativity and interpersonal relationships across and within generations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(6), 1273–1286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (2013). Scientific genius is extinct. Nature, 493(7434), 602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (2014a). Creative performance, expertise acquisition, individual differences, and developmental antecedents: An integrative research approach. Intelligence, 45, 66–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (Ed.). (2014b). The Wiley handbook of genius. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorokin, P. A. (1957). Social and cultural dynamics. Boston: Porter Sargent Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P. (2003). Historical dynamics: Why states rise and fall. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P., & Nefedov, S. A. (2009). Secular cycles. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yukawa, H. (1978). Creativity and intuition: A physicist looks at east and west. Tokyo, New York: Kodansha International.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

For advice and suggestions, thanks are due to two anonymous referees. We acknowledge financial support from the Australian Research Council (FT110100463).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benno Torgler.

Appendix

Appendix

See Figs. 4 and 5.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Eminence development by field based on mean mD

Fig. 5
figure 5

Eminence development by field based on sum mD

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chan, H.F., Torgler, B. Do great minds appear in batches?. Scientometrics 104, 475–488 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-015-1620-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-015-1620-8

Keywords

Navigation