Skip to main content
Log in

Abstract

We describe preliminary applications of network analysis techniques to eye-tracking data collected during a collaborative learning activity. This paper makes three contributions: first, we visualize collaborative eye-tracking data as networks, where the nodes of the graph represent fixations and edges represent saccades. We found that those representations can serve as starting points for formulating research questions and hypotheses about collaborative processes. Second, network metrics can be computed to interpret the properties of the graph and find proxies for the quality of students’ collaboration. We found that different characteristics of our graphs correlated with different aspects of students’ collaboration (for instance, the extent to which students reached consensus was associated with the average size of the strongly connected components of the graphs). Third, we used those characteristics to predict the quality of students’ collaboration by feeding those features into a machine-learning algorithm. We found that among the eight dimensions of collaboration that we considered, we were able to roughly predict (using a median-split) students’ quality of collaboration with an accuracy between ~85 and 100 %. We conclude by discussing implications for developing “collaboration-sensing” tools, and comment on implementing this approach for formal learning environments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (2004). Cognitive psychology and its implications (6th ed.). Worth Publishers.

  • Barron, B. (2003). When smart groups fail. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(3), 307–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catan, L. (1986). The dynamic display of process: Historical development and contemporary uses of the microgenetic method. Human Development, 29(5), 252–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherubini, M., Nüssli, M.-A., & Dillenbourg, P. (2008). Deixis and gaze in collaborative work at a distance (over a shared map): a computational model to detect misunderstandings. In Proceedings of the 2008 Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications, ETRA’08 (pp. 173–180). New York: ACM.

  • Cortes, C., & Vapnik, V. N. (1995). Support-vector networks. Machine Learning, 20(3), 273–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelbart, D. (1963). Conceptual framework for the augmentation of man’s intellect. In P. Howerton & D. Weeks (Eds.), Vistas in information handling (pp. 1–29). Spartan Books.

  • Erdos, P., & Rényi, A. (1960). On the evolution of random graphs. Publ. Math. Inst. Hung. Acad. Sci, 5, 17–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Draguns, J. (1957). A microgenetic approach to perception and thought. Psychological Bulletin, 54(3), 197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, L. (1977). A set of measures of centrality based upon betweenness. Sociometry, 40, 35–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fruchterman, T. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Software: Practice and Experience, 21(11), 1129–1164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1, 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jermann, P., Mullins, D., Nuessli, M.-A., & Dillenbourg, P. (2011). Collaborative gaze footprints: Correlates of interaction quality. In Proceedings of CSCL 2011. Hong Kong.

  • Licklider, J. C. R. (1960). Man-computer symbiosis. IRE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, 1, 4–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Hsueh, P.-Y., Lai, J., Sangin, M., Nussli, M.-A., & Dillenbourg, P. (2009). Who is the expert? Analyzing gaze data to predict expertise level in collaborative applications. In IEEE International Conference on Multimedia and Expo, 2009. ICME 2009 (pp. 898 –901).

  • Luria, A. (1978). The development of writing in the child. In M. Cole (Ed.), The selected writings of A. R. Luria. New York: M. Sharpe. Originally published, 1928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meier, A., Spada, H., & Rummel, N. (2007). A rating scheme for assessing the quality of computer-supported collaboration processes. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 2(1), 63–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (2004). The social and technological dimensions of “scaffolding” and related theoretical concepts for learning, education and human activity. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(3), 423–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., & Hilton, M. L. (Eds.). (2012). Education for life and work: Developing transferable knowledge and skills in the 21st century. Washington, DC: National Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pentland, A. S. (2010). Honest signals. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29(6), 1045–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, V. (2004). Microgenesis, immediate experience and visual processes in reading. In Seeing, thinking and knowing (pp. 221–243). Netherlands: Springer.

  • Roth, W.-M. (2001). Gestures: Their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 365–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, K. J. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology, 1(1), 43–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B., & Pea, R. (2013). Real-time mutual gaze perception enhances collaborative learning and collaboration quality. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 8(4), 375–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L. (1995). The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(3), 321–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 129–184.

  • Scribner, S. (1984). Organizing knowledge at work. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 6(1&2), 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scribner, S. (1985). Vygotsky’s uses of history. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 119–145). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selassie, D., Heller, B., & Heer, J. (2011). Divided edge bundling for directional network data. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 17(12), 2354–2363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2002). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., & LeBaron, C. D. (Eds.). (2014). Embodied Interaction: Language and body in the material world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher mental process. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1940). The comparative psychology of mental development (2nd ed.). Chicago: Follett, 1948. Originally published, 1926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1956). Microgenesis and aphasia. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 52, 347–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge grant support from the National Science Foundation (NSF) for this work from the LIFE Center (NSF #0835854).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bertrand Schneider.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table 4 Social network analysis (SNA) glossary for eye-tracking graphs

Appendix 2

Table 5 Spearman correlation matrix for network metrics and quality of collaboration (* < 0.05, ** < 0.01)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schneider, B., Pea, R. Toward collaboration sensing. Intern. J. Comput.-Support. Collab. Learn. 9, 371–395 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-014-9202-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-014-9202-y

Keywords

Navigation