Skip to main content
Log in

Theory of Mind in Social Robots: Replication of Five Established Human Tests

  • Original research
  • Published:
International Journal of Social Robotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory of Mind is an inferential system central to human–human communication by which people ascribe mental states to self and other, and then use those deductions to make predictions about others’ behaviors. Despite the likelihood that ToM may also be central to interactions with other types of agents exhibiting similar cues, it is not yet fully known whether humans develop ToM for mechanical agents exhibiting properties of intelligence and sociality. A suite of five tests for implicit ToM were performed (white lie test, behavioral intention task, facial affect inference, vocal affect inference, and false-belief test) for three different robots and a human control. Findings suggest that implicit ToM signals are consistent across variably human-like robots and humans, so long as the social cues are similar and interpretable, but there is no association between implicit ToM signals and explicit mind ascription; findings suggest that heuristics and deliberation of mental status of robots may compete with implicit social-cognitive reactions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Interrater reliability metrics, in accounting for chance agreement, are punishing when the phenomenon is infrequent [49]. Because (a) mentalistic explanations for intentionality and (b) emotional state explanations for facial affect each occurred in only ~ 10–20% of the sample, percent agreement is taken here as an acceptable metric for interrater reliability.

  2. A robust test of equality of means could not be conducted here because the low-anthropomorphic robot condition featured no variance—no respondent in that condition detected sarcasm.

References

  1. Schilbach L, Wohlschlaeger AM, Kraemer NC, Newen A, Shah AJ, Fink GR, Vogeley K (2006) Being with virtual others: neural correlates of social interactions. Neuropsychologia 44:718–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Apperly IA (2012) What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes, and individual differences. Q J Exp Psychol 65:825–839

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Kasperbauer TJ (2017) Mentalizing animals: implications for moral psychology and animal ethics. Philos Stud 174:465–484

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Nass C, Steuer J, Tauber ER (1994) Computers are social actors. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. Boston, MA

  6. Reeves B, Nass C (1996) The media equation: how people treat computers, television, and new media like real people and places. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bartneck C, Kulić D, Croft E, Zoghbi S (2009) Measurement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceived intelligence, and perceived safety of robots. Int J Soc Robot 1:71–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Epley N, Waytz A, Cacioppo JT (2007) On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychol Rev 114:864–886

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Gordon RM (1986) Folk psychology as simulation. Mind Lang 8:101–118

    Google Scholar 

  10. Goldman AI (2012) Theory of mind. In: Margolis E, Samuels R, Stich S (eds) Oxford handbook of philosophy and cognitive science. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 402–424

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  11. Slaughter V (2015) Theory of mind in infants and young children: a review. Aust Psychol 50:169–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Knudsen B, Liszkowski U (2012) Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Dev Sci 15:113–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Schulte-Ruther M, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR, Piefke M (2007) Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy. J Cognit Neurosci 19:1354–1372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Sodian B, Taylor C, Harris PL, Perner J (1991) Early deception and the child’s theory of mind: false trails and genuine markers. Child Dev 62:468–483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J (1999) Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: a critique of the ‘social skills deficit’ view of anti-social behaviour. Soc Dev 8:117–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gopnik A (1998) Explanation as orgasm. Mind Mach 8:101–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Heal J (1986) Replication and functionalism. In: Butterfield J (ed) Language, mind, and logic. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 135–150

    Google Scholar 

  18. Harris PL, Johnson CN, Hutton D, Andrews G, Cooke T (1989) Young children’s theory of mind and emotion. Cogn Emot 3:379–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Low J, Perner J (2012) Implicit and explicit theory of mind: state of the art. Br J Dev Psychol 30:1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Eyssel FA, Pfundmair M (2015) Predictors of psychological anthropomorphization, mind perception, and the fulfillment of social needs: a case study with a zoomorphic robot. In: RO-MAN: 24th IEEE international symposium on in robot and human interactive communication, Kobe, Japan

  21. Martini MC, Buzzell GA, Wiese E (2015) Agent appearance modulates mind attribution and social attention in human–robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the international conference on social robotics, Paris, France

  22. Tanibe T, Hashimoto T, Karasawa K (2017) We perceive a mind in a robot when we help it. PLoS ONE 12, article e0180952

  23. Gray HM, Gray K, Wegner DM (2007) Dimensions of mind perception. Science 315(5812):619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR (1995) Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev 102:4–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Hofmann W, Gawronski B, Gschwendner T, Le H, Schmitt M (2005) A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 31:1369–1385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. de Graaf MMA, Malle BF (2019) People’s explanations of robot behavior subtly reveal mental state inferences. In: Proceedings of the international conference on human–robot interaction, HRI’19, Daegu, South Korea

  27. Byom LJ, Mutlu B (2013) Theory of mind: mechanisms, methods, and new directions. Front Hum Neurosci 7, article 413

  28. Happé FGE (1994) An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord 2:129–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Safarti Y, Hardy-Baylé MC, Besche C, Widlöcher D (1997) Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: a non-verbal exploration with comic strips. Schizophr Res 25:199–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Baron-Cohen S (2001) Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme 34:74–83

    Google Scholar 

  31. McDonald S, Bornhofen C, Shum D, Long E, Saunders C, Neulinger K (2006) Reliability and validity of the awareness of social inference test (tasit): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil Rehabil 28:1529–1542

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13:103–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Keysar B, Lin S, Barr DJ (2003) Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition 89:25–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Jack RE, Garrod OG, Schyns PG (2014) Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Curr Biol 24:187–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Krippendorf K (2004) Reliability in content analysis. Hum Commun Res 30:411–433

    Google Scholar 

  36. Ekman P (1993) Facial expression and emotion. Am Psychol 48:384–392

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Meinhardt-Injac B, Daum MM, Meinhardt G, Persike M (2013) The two-systems account of theory of mind: testing the links to social-perceptual and cognitive abilities. Front Hum Neurosci 12:25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Edwards C, Edwards AP, Spence PR, Westerman DK (2016) Initial interaction expectations with robots: testing the human-to-human interaction script. Commun Stud 67:227–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Bryson JJ (2010) Robots should be slaves. In: Wilks Y (ed) Close engagements with artificial companions: key social, psychological, ethical and design issues. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 63–74

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  40. Sundar SS (2008) The MAIN model: a heuristic approach to understanding technology effects on credibility. In: Metzger MJ, Flanagin AJ (eds) Digital media, youth, and credibility. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 73–100

    Google Scholar 

  41. Lee S-L, Kiesler S, Lau IY-M, Chiu C-Y (2005) Human mental models of humanoid robots. In: Proceedings of the 2005 IEEE international conference on robotics and automation, Barcelona, Spain, pp 2767–2772

  42. Sundar SS, Waddell TF, Jung EH (2016) The hollywood robot syndrome: media effects on older adults’ attitudes toward robots and adoption intentions. In: The 11th ACM/IEE international conference on human robot interaction, HRI’16, Christchurch, New Zealand

  43. Mara M, Appel M (2015) Science fiction reduces the eeriness of android robots: a field experiment. Comput Hum Behav 48:156–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Schreiner C, Mara M, Appel M (2017) When R2-D2 hops off the screen: a service robot encountered in real life appears more real and human-like than on video or in VR. In: Proceedings of MediaPsych2017: the 10th conference of the media psychology division of the German Psychological Society, Landau, Germany

  45. Banks J (2019) A perceived moral agency scale: development and validation of a metric for humans and social machines. Comput Hum Behav 90:363–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Gray K, Wegner DM (2012) Feeling robots and human zombies: mind perception and the uncanny valley. Cognition 125:125–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Stafford RQ, MacDonald BA, Jayawardena C, Wegner DM, Broadbent E (2014) Does the robot have a mind? Mind perception and attitudes towards robots predict use of an eldercare robot. Int J Soc Robot 6:17–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. de Graaf M (2016) An ethical evaluation of human–robot relationships. Int J Soc Robot 8:589–598

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Quarfoot D, Levine RA (2016) How robust are multirater interrater reliability indices to changes in frequency distribution. Am Stat 70:373–384

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author gratefully acknowledges the contributions of Tiffany A. Dykstra-DeVette and Katie Burgess in creating stimulus materials, of Mia Del Borrello and Tai Nguyen in data coding, and of Nick Bowman’s input on this manuscript. Thanks also to West Virginia University Department of Communication, where a portion of this work was conducted.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jaime Banks.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Banks, J. Theory of Mind in Social Robots: Replication of Five Established Human Tests. Int J of Soc Robotics 12, 403–414 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00588-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-019-00588-x

Keywords

Navigation