Skip to main content
Log in

Landmarks are Exaggerated

  • Discussion
  • Published:
KI - Künstliche Intelligenz Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this discussion essay, I contend that the role of landmarks is exaggerated in basic and applied spatial cognition research. Specifically, I discuss empirical and theoretical arguments consistent with two claims. First, the word landmark is a label for several different concepts, although its precise reference in a particular context is rarely specified carefully. Further, whether specified or not, researchers never use the term landmark to mean everything that the concept can legitimately mean. Thus, when researchers assert something about the role of landmarks in spatial cognitive activities, they exaggerate their particular meaning at the expense of a broader ontology. Second, I claim that even when landmarks are clearly and precisely defined, their role in specifying location is misunderstood and less fundamental than proposed. In exaggerating landmarks, other important components of spatial knowledge, memory, and reasoning are undervalued. Taken together, these two claims support my contention that landmarks are exaggerated in spatial cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. They must be remembered as a specific token, not just a type. Not “a house” but “Leila’s House” (if there are no other houses around, a house becomes a token object).

  2. Of course, the physical object that materially instantiates a sign can function as a landmark instead of a semiotic artifact—“go past the metal pole holding the red six-sided object”.

  3. Formally defined geometric terms like “metric” do not strictly describe human spatial knowledge, as violations of the metric axioms, etc., are easily demonstrated. I use the term informally to suggest that humans display considerably more spatial knowledge than just nonmetric properties like connectivity and sequence.

References

  1. Allen GL, Kirasic KC, Siegel AW, Herman JF (1979) Developmental issues in cognitive mapping: the selection and utilization of environmental landmarks. Child Dev 56:1062–1070

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Appleyard D, Lynch K, Myer JR (1964) The view from the road. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  3. Arthur P, Passini R (1992) Wayfinding: people, signs, and architecture. McGraw-Hill Ryerson, Toronto

    Google Scholar 

  4. Benedikt M, Burnham CA (1985) Perceiving architectural space: from optic arrays to isovists. In: Warren WH, Shaw RE (eds) Persistence and change, Proc First Inter Conf Event Perc. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 103–114

    Google Scholar 

  5. Burnett G (2000) ‘Turn right at the traffic lights’: the requirement for landmarks in vehicle navigation systems. J Navig 53:499–510

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cheng K (1986) A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition 23:149–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Collett TS (1996) Insect navigation en route to the goal: multiple strategies for the use of landmarks. J Exp Bio 199:227–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Cornell EH, Heth CD, Broda LS (1989) Children’s wayfinding: response to instructions to use environmental landmarks. Dev Psychol 25:755–764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Couclelis H, Golledge RG, Gale N, Tobler W (1987) Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition. J Environ Psychol 7:99–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Denis M, Michon P-E, Tom A (2007) Assisting pedestrian wayfinding in urban settings: why references to landmarks are crucial in direction-giving. In: Allen GL (ed) Applied spatial cognition: from research to cognitive technology. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 25–51

    Google Scholar 

  11. Duckham M, Winter S, Robinson M (2010) Including landmarks in routing instructions. J Locat Based Serv 4:28–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dyer FC, Gould JL (1983) Honey bee navigation. Am Sci 71:587–597

    Google Scholar 

  13. Foo P, Warren WH, Duchon A, Tarr MJ (2005) Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 31:195–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Gallistel CR (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press, Cambridge

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  15. Gibson JJ (1950) The perception of the visual world. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  17. Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  18. Gladwin T (1970) East is a big bird. Harvard University, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  19. Golledge RG (1999) Human wayfinding and cognitive maps. In: Golledge RG (ed) Wayfinding behavior: cognitive mapping and other spatial processes. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore, pp 5–45

    Google Scholar 

  20. Hermer L, Spelke E (1996) Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation. Cognition 61:195–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Hirtle SC (2011) Geographical design: spatial cognition and geographical information science. Morgan & Claypool, San Rafael

    Google Scholar 

  22. Ishikawa T, Montello DR (2006) Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places. Cognit Psychol 52:93–129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ittelson WH (1973) Environment perception and contemporary perceptual theory. In: Ittelson WH (ed) Environment and cognition. Seminar, New York, pp 1–19

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kuipers B (2000) The spatial semantic hierarchy. Artif Intell 119:191–233

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  25. Kuipers B, Tecuci DG, Stankiewicz BJ (2003) The skeleton in the cognitive map: a computational and empirical exploration. Environ Behav 35:81–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Lynch K (1960) The image of the city. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  27. Mark DM, Freksa C, Hirtle SC, Lloyd R, Tversky B (1999) Cognitive models of geographical space. Int J Geogr Inf Sci 13:747–774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Miller J, Carlson L (2011) Selecting landmarks in novel environments. Psychon Bull Rev 18:184–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Montello DR (1998) A new framework for understanding the acquisition of spatial knowledge in large-scale environments. In: Egenhofer MJ, Golledge RG (eds) Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems. Oxford University, New York, pp 143–154

    Google Scholar 

  30. Montello DR, Raubal M (2012) Functions and applications of spatial cognition. In: Waller D, Nadel L (eds) Handbook of spatial cognition. American Psychological Association, Washington DC, pp 249–264

    Google Scholar 

  31. Presson CC, Montello DR (1988) Points of reference in spatial cognition: stalking the elusive landmark. Br J Dev Psychol 6:378–381

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Raubal M, Winter S (2002) Enriching wayfinding instructions with local landmarks. In: Egenhofer MJ, Mark DM (eds) Geog Infor Sci, Second Inter Conf GISci 2002, Lect Notes Comp Sci 2478. Springer, Berlin, pp 243–259

    Google Scholar 

  33. Richter K-F, Winter S (2014) Landmarks: GIScience for intelligent services. Springer, Berlin

    Book  Google Scholar 

  34. Sadalla EK, Burroughs WJ, Staplin LJ (1980) Reference points in spatial cognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 6:516–528

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Siegel AW, White SH (1975) The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. In: Reese HW (ed) Advances in child development and behavior, vol 10. Academic, New York, pp 9–55

    Google Scholar 

  36. Sorrows ME, Hirtle SC (1999) The nature of landmarks for real and electronic spaces. In: Freksa C, Mark DM (eds) Spatial information theory. Springer, Berlin, pp 37–50

    Google Scholar 

  37. Steck SD, Mallot HA (2000) The role of global and local landmarks in virtual environment navigation. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 9:69–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55:189–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Trowbridge CC (1913) On fundamental methods of orientation and “imaginary maps”. Science 38:888–897

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Yeap WK, Jefferies ME (1999) Computing a representation of the local environment. Artif Intell 107:265–301

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

An earlier version of this essay was originally given as an oral presentation on July 5, 2010, at the international meeting “Las Navas 2010: Cognitive and Linguistic Aspects of Geographic Space”, held at Las Navas del Marques, Avila, Spain.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel R. Montello.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Montello, D.R. Landmarks are Exaggerated. Künstl Intell 31, 193–197 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13218-016-0473-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13218-016-0473-5

Keywords

Navigation