Elsevier

Neural Networks

Volume 15, Issue 3, April 2002, Pages 309-326
Neural Networks

Contributed article
A control model of the movement of attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00024-2Get rights and content

Abstract

A control model of the movement of the focus of attention is developed and applied to explain its observed effects on single cell activity and to various quantitative features of the Posner benefit paradigm. This supports the presence of an inverse controller and a rules component in the control model. The ability of the control model to explain a range of deficits is then analyzed, as is its relation to other modeling approaches.

Introduction

There is presently great interest in attention from both a psychological and a neuro-scientific point of view. In the psychological approach, a number of paradigms have been used to measure reaction time and success or error levels in human response to determine the nature of attention and how it is controlled (Pashler, 1998a, Pashler, 1998b, Wright, 1998, Parasuraman, 1999). Neuro-scientific techniques have been developed more recently, associated with brain imaging (PET, fMRI, MEG and EEG) or single cell measurements, to find sites in the brain and their dynamical flow of activity as attention is controlled (Kastner and Unegerleider, 2000, Reynolds et al., 1999). The use of brain imaging and single cell recording has now become ever more closely linked to psychological paradigms. At the same time a better understanding of brain processing, including attention, has been obtained by analysis of the deficits brought about by stroke or other brain damage, as well as in cases of mental disability of a neuro-chemical nature. Use of brain imaging for these cases, and comparison with normal subjects, has led to a better understanding of attention.

Attention is seen as composed of a number of separate parts: alerting and preparation for response, vigilance and the holding of attention, and movement of the attention focus. Brain sites active during these various processes are being uncovered by brain imaging, and the detailed topography of the attention movement system, in particular, is now becoming clear, it being composed of at least two separate systems:

  • 1.

    The ‘plant’ (in control terms), consisting of early cortical sites (and associated sub-cortical thalamic and nuclearis-reticularis thalami regions) in which feature analysis and object coding occur. Attention control is now reasonably well accepted as achieving the process of relative amplification of neural activity at the attended sites and reduction of it at unattended places (Reynolds et al., 1999). How extensive is this modulation in cortex is still being determined, both for unimodal input sites and those for output response (Romo & Salinas, 2001).

  • 2.

    The controller of the movement of attention, determining where this relative amplification/reduction occurs (and how), is in separate sites to the plant (Vidyasagar, 1999, Kastner and Unegerleider, 2000). There has been controversy about the site of this function: components are now recognized as in the parietal and frontal lobes (Wright, 1998, Kastner and Unegerleider, 2000). However how control is divided amongst modalities is still undetermined.

It is also controversial that a possible third component is present, acting as a state estimator, in other words as an ‘observer’ or forward model in the control sense (Jacobs, 1993, Franklin et al., 1989). There is a set of prefrontal modules (anterior cingulate, SEF, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, SMA) which have been noted (Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000 and references therein) as part of the attention control system acting in a manner independent of input. This network could act as an observer in the attention control system to achieve more rapid attention control than by waiting for afferent feedback to arrive at the control signal generator from the amplified signal. At the same time sites in parietal, playing the role of working memory buffers, could also be important components of any such observer system. They would presumably function as part of state updating. However, the existence of an observer is controversial, and ultimately is to be tackled by attention modeling, combined with further experimentation. A preliminary discussion is given by Taylor, 2000a, Taylor, 2000b (see also Taylor, 2001b).

The division of attention into a separate automatic ‘exogenous’ and a controlled ‘endogenous’ part is also now well established (Pashler, 1998b, Wright, 1998, Parasuraman, 1999, Kastner and Unegerleider, 2000). The automatic control component is based on modules sited in the parietal and sub-cortical regions (especially the superior colliculus), whilst the prefrontal and parietal modules mentioned earlier (Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000) are employed for endogenous attention control to be achieved. It is supposed that top-down control is governed by components in working memory, since attention and working memory are realized to be closely linked (Coull & Frith, 1998). To understand attention, we must also improve our comprehension of working memory; as noted earlier it plays an important role in any putative observer system for more efficient control. Evidence has recently been presented for identification of the controllers for endogenous and exogenous attention (Pashler, 1998b); these will be considered in more detail in a later section.

There have been numerous attempts to model attention by neural networks (Mozer and Sitton, 1999, Koch and Ullman, 1985, Lee et al., 1999, Fellenz, 1994, Deco, 1998). Some neural models use a specific control signal, generated by a competitive process on a separate module, to relatively amplify the inputs to earlier sites. There are also attempts to include prefrontal sites as template holders; as such memory-based rule processing is one of the functions of that area (Jackson, Marrocco, & Posner, 1994). Here we will systematically use engineering control theory as a framework for analysis of the attention process as a whole. The purpose of the present paper is thus to present the simulation of a neural model, based on a control framework, for the movement of attention under either exogenous or endogenous control. It will be tested on a range of known single cell, brain imaging and deficit data, with the results of simulations being compared with experimental data.

A control approach to attention is justified by the following.

  • Such a framework is important in understanding how the brain enables motor control to be so effective, in particular, so fast (Miall & Wolpert, 1996). Engineering models of control allow functionality to be assigned to brain regions in a manner difficult to understand using the standard neural modeling approach in which it is hoped that distinct functionality would arise automatically by learning in a set of modules initially without specific connections.

  • Higher cognitive functions should not be considered separately from other bodily functions. If motor actions on ‘muscular’ parts of the body can be more efficiently viewed under the framework of engineering control theory then the same should be true for other processing performed by the brain in which transformations are performed by one neural system on another—the crux of thinking (Chomsky, 1975).

  • The most influential psychological models of attention are those of Posner et al., 1987, LaBerge and Brown, 1989, Desimone and Duncan, 1995. None of these models explicitly uses an engineering control framework, but we will see later that these models are compatible with the control approach we will develop.

Let us consider that last point more fully. We start with the model of Posner et al. (1987), who proposed a dissociation of attention movement into various subcomponents. The sequence of processes involved was suggested as:alertinterruptlocalizedisengagemoveengageinhibitEvidence has been presented in a series of papers by Posner and colleagues to support such a dissociation with various brain sites being put forward to support the separate components of the process I (Posner, 1988, Posner et al., 1995). All of these involve control of neural activity by various modules: alerting, interrupting, localizing, moving, and so on. Thus the Posner model incorporates attention movement as controlled by signals from specific modules, consistent with an engineering control approach. The model of LaBerge and Brown (1989) is based on the selection of an area in visual space, using both excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms in a filter module to enable object identification. Subsequent movement of attention is handled by the presence of a processing gradient. The location filter is guided in its selection by a higher order goal system, and thereby biases feature flow to an object module. Yet again the model of LaBerge and Brown (1989) is of control form. Finally the influential model of Desimone and Duncan (1995) appears on the surface not to involve any control structures. As they say (p. 194) “Instead the model we develop is that attention is an emergent property of many neural mechanisms working together to resolve competition for visual processing and control of behavior”. The model uses biased competition between various object representations, as based on related single cell observations in anterior temporal cortex in monkeys performing saccades to object shapes matching previously presented ones among a set of distracters. The bias may be bottom-up, such as brought about in exogenous attention control by sudden onset, or can arise from a top-down goal to observe some target feature such as a specific color or to observe a particular object such as a letter X, or from a spatial location bias. This approach is yet again an example of a control system, contrary to what would appear from the quotation, with attention control signals either being bottom-up or top-down. We will turn to a more complete discussion of the control aspects of these approaches later in the paper.

From the earlier comments, especially in association with the above three influential psychological models of attention, it is reasonable to use engineering control as a general framework inside which to construct a model of attention. This will especially allow quantitative testing of the control approach to attention movement, and more specifically justify the presence or absence of specific components of the overall control model. We start in Section 2 with a description of the basis of the model and its mathematical formulation. In Section 2, we turn to describe single cell data, which strongly support the feedback modulation hypothesis for the basic action of attention on sensory cortical areas. Analysis of the higher order control aspects are then tackled, starting in Section 4 with the discussion of the Posner paradigm for the dependence of valid and invalid cues for direction of attention on values of the inter-stimulus interval. The Posner paradigm allows the mechanisms involved in the shifting of attention to be probed. Detailed simulation results of both the single cell effects and the Posner benefit paradigm are presented in Section 5. The effects on these benefit results of altered inhibition in monkeys as well as certain human deficits are compared with experimental data in Section 6. In Section 7, there is a comparison with other models of attention. The paper finishes with a brief summary of results and a discussion of extension of the model to further paradigms.

Section snippets

Basis of the model

The approach to modeling attention taken here, as noted in Section 1, is in terms of a control framework. It is natural to make the following assumption on this basis:

Attention is a feedback control system, with the options of bottom-up (exogenous) or top-down (endogenous) control signals. These control signals amplify a selected component of the input so as to achieve more effective processing in the presence of distracters. The control system involves an inverse control model, together with a

Simulation approach to the IMC/plant/rules modules

There is experimental data allowing support and development of the model of Fig. 2 and discussed in Section 2. We consider in this section the generation of the control signal able to achieve feedback amplification. To probe this further, let us turn to relevant experimental data. Neural activity in early sites in visual cortex is known to be modulated by attention. A considerable body of data exists from brain imaging on this (Kim et al., 1999). It allows for placing of the various modules of

Simulation approach to the Posner paradigm and the rule module

The Posner paradigm, described in Kim et al. (1999), explores the manner in which attention is moved either exogenously or endogenously. The subject views a lighted screen on which stimuli appear. They are required to fixate on a central cross throughout the experiment. A cue appears, either by the brightening of a stimulus box to left or right of the fixation point (exogenous attention direction), or by a central arrow pointing to left or right (endogenous attention direction), to direct

Results of the simulations of the Reynolds experiment

The first experiment to be simulated was that of Reynolds et al. (1999). The time course of activity in the neuron described in Fig. 3 and , , , , was plotted and compared with results from single-cell recordings. The parameters A (a decay constant) and B (which determines the maximum firing rate of the neuron) were varied to adjust the shape of the time course until it matched the published results as well as possible.

The first values assigned to A and B were those used in the simulations by

Deficit modeling

One of the crucial bases for the disengage–move–engage model of Posner discussed in Section 1, as well as of other approaches, is the effect of brain deficits on attention processing. It is therefore appropriate for us to consider how the general control model we have presented so far can explain details observed both in humans and other animals. We consider here two sets of results of this class: that of Petersen, Robinson, and Morris (1987) in which the effects were determined in monkeys of

Comparison with other models

There is an increasing number of models of attention, and it is important to relate our control model to them and in particular to compare and contrast the underlying approaches so as to assess the benefits obtained from our control approach to attention. We will start by considering models of information flow based on psychological and neuro-scientific experimental results. We already briefly discussed in Section 1 the psychology-based models of Posner and Cohen, 1984, LaBerge and Brown, 1989,

Conclusions and future work

In conclusion, we have presented a control model of the movement of attention, identifying modules involved: plant, IMC, rules, buffer, monitor, and other components of an observer. We have given experimental support for the existence of the first three of these components, using single cell data (Reynolds et al., 1999) and psychophysical data on the Posner benefit (Wright, 1998). We have further supported the control model by analysis of deficits in monkeys (Petersen et al., 1987) and humans (

References (43)

  • G. Deco

    Biased competition mechanisms for visual attention in a multimodular neurodynamics system

  • R. Desimone et al.

    Neural mechanics of selective visual attention

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (1995)
  • J. Duncan et al.

    Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision

    Nature

    (1994)
  • Fellenz, W. (1994). A sequential model for attentive object selection. Proceedings of the 39th IWK Conference,...
  • G.F. Franklin et al.

    Digital control of dynamic systems

    (1989)
  • F.J. Friedrich et al.

    Effects of parietal lobe injury on covert orienting of visual attention

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1984)
  • R. Hari et al.

    Left mini-neglect in dyslexic adults

    Brain

    (2001)
  • O.L.R. Jacobs

    An introduction to control theory

    (1993)
  • S. Kastner et al.

    Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Kim, Y. -H., Gileman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., La Bar, K. S., & Mesulam, H. -M. (1999). The large-scale...
  • C. Koch et al.

    Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry

    Human Neurobiololgy

    (1985)
  • Cited by (50)

    • Does the corollary discharge of attention exist?

      2012, Consciousness and Cognition
    • Resolving some confusions over attention and consciousness

      2007, Neural Networks
      Citation Excerpt :

      These interaction are now being observed in the attention blink paradigm (Sergent et al., 2005), as discussed in the next section. Other attention phenomena that can be explained in terms of reduced versions of the CODAM mode are: the Posner attention paradigm (Taylor & Rogers, 2002), working memory rehearsal (Korsten, Fragopanagos, Hartley, Taylor, & Taylor, 2006) and the N2pc as well as numerous other attention tasks as demonstrated by the modelling through the other models mentioned earlier, as regarded as simpler versions of CODAM. More detailed aspects of the feedback control have also been studied at the micro level (Taylor, 2006), which implies that the attention feedback is of sigma-pi or multiplicative form.

    • Attention as a controller

      2006, Neural Networks
      Citation Excerpt :

      However this architecture was not so strongly linked to a visual attention control model such as CODAM, so that in order to build a more complete framework for attention as a basis for cognitive processing, we need to develop an architecture for reasoning included in a CODAM attention framework. There is clear need for motor attention control, and previous work in Fragopanagos et al. (2005) and Taylor and Fragopanagos (2003) proposed a model of motor attention as an extension of the ballistic control model of attention of Taylor and Rogers (2002), and applied it successfully to a number of visuomotor paradigms. This motor attention model was fused with a similar ballistic control model of visual processing by means of direct transmission of visual information to the IMC of the motor attention control model.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text