Elsevier

Computers in Human Behavior

Volume 80, March 2018, Pages 59-66
Computers in Human Behavior

Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.018Get rights and content

Highlights

  • IGD adolescents' DD behaviors reflect an inability to delay gratification.

  • IGD adolescents' PD behaviors reflect a normal risk-taking tendency.

  • Impulsivity might act as a behavioral marker of adolescents with IGD.

Abstract

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is a behavioral addiction that is gradually becoming a public health problem. We aimed to identify the behavioral characteristics of adolescents with IGD to promote early diagnosis and intervention. As impulsivity and risk taking are the most important traits of addiction, we used several representative methods, including a delay discounting task, a probability discounting task and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11), to measure the impulsivity and risk-taking tendency of adolescents with IGD and a comparison group. Consistent with previous findings on problematic Internet use (PIU), our results indicated that adolescents with IGD had a larger degree of delay discounting (i.e., more impulsive decision making), regardless of the outcome amount and valence, and higher scores on all three subscales of the BIS-11 than the comparison group. However, there was no intergroup difference in the probability discounting task. In general, the current research posits that impulsive decision making and personality traits, but not risk taking, might co-occur with IGD in adolescents.

Introduction

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is defined as the persistent and recurrent use of the Internet to engage in games, often with other players. It leads to clinically significant impairments (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) such as insomnia (Lam, 2014), intensified social anxiety (Lo, Wang, & Fang, 2005), elevated levels of depression (Romer, Bagdasarov, & More, 2013), and even suicidal behaviors (Kaess et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2016). IGD is highly prevalent worldwide, particularly among adolescents (Petry, Rehbein, Ko, & O'Brien, 2015). As IGD is increasingly recognized as a global public health problem, the fifth revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) included it as a tentative disorder to foster more research for a better understanding of IGD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Adolescence is a critical period of rapid brain development and behavioral maturation, and adolescents are vulnerable to various types of addictive behaviors (Casey, Jones, & Hare, 2008). Studies have repeatedly shown that most substance users and problem/pathological gamblers begin consuming drugs or gambling during adolescence (Andersen and Teicher, 2009, Giovino, 1999, O'Loughlin et al., 2009, Yip et al., 2011), and the acute and long-term effects of drug use can cause more damage for adolescents than for adults (Meier et al., 2012, Thoma et al., 2011). Moreover, the intensity of alcohol use and gambling in adolescence is correlated with the severity of alcohol and gambling problems in adulthood (Burge et al., 2004, McCambridge et al., 2011). In terms of IGD, adolescents should receive special attention as the prevalence rates of IGD are much higher among adolescents than among adults (Festl et al., 2013, Rehbein et al., 2015). In some studies, the rates of IGD among adolescents exceed 8% (King et al., 2013, Mentzoni et al., 2011). To promote the prevention of and early intervention for IGD, the first step is understanding its etiology by identifying the behavioral traits of adolescents with IGD.

Impulsivity and risk taking are the most important behavioral traits associated with addiction (Everitt and Robbins, 2016, Kreek et al., 2005, Ryan et al., 2013). Impulsivity is defined as “a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli with diminished regard to the negative consequences” (Potenza & de Wit, 2010). It is a multidimensional construct that comprises the impulsive personality trait, impulsive decision making and impulsive motor responses (M. R. Mitchell and Potenza, 2014, Weafer et al., 2014). Impulsivity is regarded as the endophenotype of addiction because it is involved in the initiation, maintenance, and relapse of drug use (Everitt and Robbins, 2016, Jentsch et al., 2014). In parallel, risk taking is the inclination to engage in behaviors that are performed under uncertainty regardless of the possible negative consequences (Kreek et al., 2005). Many studies have shown that risk taking is linked to various addictive behaviors (Fernie et al., 2010, Lejuez et al., 2005, MacPherson et al., 2010, Spurrier and Blaszczynski, 2014). Risk preference is the core feature of pathological gambling. Overall, almost every type of addictive behavior is associated with impulsivity, risk taking or both, and these two traits persistently characterize the behaviors of adolescents (Casey et al., 2008a, Steinberg, 2008). Therefore, for adolescents with IGD, the following question arises: Is excessive online gaming associated with elevated impulsivity or a higher risk-taking tendency?

Some studies have used multiple methods to investigate whether adolescents and adults with IGD exhibit more impulsiveness and a higher risk-taking tendency than those without IGD. First, regarding impulsivity, adolescents with IGD scored higher on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), suggesting that they have a heightened impulsive personality trait (Arain et al., 2013; X. Du et al., 2016, Feng et al., 2013, Qi et al., 2015). In contrast, no distinction was found between adolescents with IGD and those without IGD in a Go/No-Go task as the two groups presented similar degrees of motor impulsivity (Arain et al., 2013). Similar to adolescents with IGD, adults with IGD scored higher on both the BIS-11 and the Arnett Inventory of Sensation Seeking (AISS) than adults without IGD (Choi et al., 2014, Ko et al., 2015, Mehroof and Griffiths, 2010). In laboratory tasks, two studies found that adults with IGD presented no deficit in motor response inhibition on the Go/No-Go task (Chen et al., 2015, Ko et al., 2014). It can thus be concluded that compared with those without IGD, adolescents and adults with IGD present elevated degrees of impulsive personality trait and similar degrees of motor impulsivity. However, impulsivity is a multidimensional construct, and the aforementioned study that used the Go/No-Go task measured only the subjects’ motor impulsivity (Arain et al., 2013). It remains unclear whether adolescents with IGD are more likely to make impulsive choices in decision-making tasks.

In addition to assessing impulsivity, different laboratory tasks have been implemented to investigate risk taking among adolescents and adults with IGD. Adults with IGD were more willing than those without IGD to take risks in both the Cups Task and the Game of Dice Task (Pawlikowski and Brand, 2011, Yao et al., 2015). Regarding risk taking among adolescents with IGD, a study using the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) reached a somewhat ambiguous conclusion (Qi et al., 2015). In that study, although adolescents with IGD made more total pumps than controls, there was no significant intergroup difference in the two indexes used as the primary measures of risk-taking tendency (i.e., the average adjusted pumps and the number of total pops) (Lejuez et al., 2002). Consequently, additional suitable paradigms should be utilized to clarify the associations between IGD and impulsiveness and risk taking among adolescents.

Delay discounting is the most commonly used measure of impulsive decision making (de Wit, 2009). It refers to the devaluation of a future reward (gain) as a function of the delay before its receipt (Ainslie, 1975, Rachlin et al., 1991). In a typical delay discounting task, subjects are instructed to make a series of binary choices between smaller, faster gains and larger, slower gains to calculate their unique delay discounting rate, which represents their preference for the smaller, faster gains and is akin to the ability to delay gratification (MacKillop et al., 2011). The higher a person's impulsivity level is, the larger his delay discounting rate will be. Delay discounting is considered a behavioral marker for addiction (Bickel, Koffarnus, Moody, & Wilson, 2014). Research has repeatedly demonstrated that addictive behaviors are associated with higher delay discounting rates in adults and adolescents (MacKillop et al., 2011). Several longitudinal studies have found that the delay discounting rate can predict both the initial foray into drug use and future abstinence (Audrain-McGovern et al., 2009, Mueller et al., 2009). In parallel, probability discounting is a phenomenon whereby the subjective value of an uncertain reward declines as the odds against receiving it increase (Green and Myerson, 2004, Rachlin et al., 1991). Though similar to delay discounting in terms of definition and experimental procedure, probability discounting is utilized to measure risk taking. In probability discounting tasks, subjects are presented with choices between smaller, certain gains and larger, probabilistic gains. Four studies using a probability discounting task consistently reported that gamblers had lower probability discounting rates than controls, suggesting that gamblers have an elevated risk-taking propensity and revealing the robust validity of the probability discounting task for detecting risk preference (Holt et al., 2003, Ligneul et al., 2013, Madden et al., 2009, Miedl et al., 2012).

Although delay discounting and probability discounting of gains have been widely examined in addiction research, most studies have focused only on the gain condition and have neglected decision-making processes that involve losses. The valence of the outcome has significant impacts on choices. People discount losses to a smaller degree than they do gains in both types of discounting tasks; this phenomenon is referred to as the “sign effect” (or valence effect) (Estle, Green, Myerson, & Holt, 2006). The delay discounting and probability discounting of losses play an important role in daily addictive behaviors because the decision to use drugs or to play games has future, possibly negative, consequences. A few experimental studies have found that cigarette smokers present larger delay discounting rates than nonsmokers for both money and health losses, suggesting their ignorance of the future in a loss condition (Baker et al., 2003, Johnson et al., 2007, Odum et al., 2002). Only one study revealed that smokers did not present a higher risk tendency than non-smokers in a probability discounting task of losses (Ohmura, Takahashi, & Kitamura, 2005). The amount of the outcome also affects decision making in both delay discounting and probability discounting tasks. People are more impulsive and more willing to take risks for small outcomes than for large outcomes, a phenomenon called the “magnitude effect” (or amount effect) (Estle et al., 2006). Heightened impulsivity and risk-taking tendency have been observed in people with addictive behaviors in discounting tasks using wide range of outcome amounts (Holt et al., 2003, MacKillop et al., 2011).

To our knowledge, no research has systematically investigated the impulsiveness and risk preferences of adolescents with IGD for both gains and losses. The current study compared adolescents with IGD with a comparison group using the monetary delay discounting task and the probability discounting task in both gain and loss conditions and with both large and small amounts. Previous studies revealed that adults with problematic Internet use (PIU) had higher delay discounting rates than adults without PIU and that there was no group difference for the probability discounting task (Q. Li et al., 2016, Saville et al., 2010). Considering that adolescents with IGD might share multiple traits with adults with PIU, two hypotheses were tested in the current study. First, we hypothesized that adolescents with IGD may be less sensitive to delayed outcomes and may show greater delay discounting than adolescents in a comparison group. Second, we hypothesized that adolescents with IGD may have an intact tendency to take risks and may demonstrate probability discounting similar to that of the comparison group.

Section snippets

Diagnostic approaches and subjects

Forty-seven adolescents with IGD were recruited from the Addiction Medicine Center, General Hospital of Beijing Military Region. They met the DSM-V criteria for Internet gaming disorder, as determined by an experienced psychiatrist, and answered “yes” to at least five questions on Young's Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addiction (YDQ) (Young, 1998). Forty-one students from a local high school were recruited as the comparison group, and their YDQ scores were no higher than four. The

Results

For the delay discounting data, the ANOVA revealed significant group, valence, and amount effects. Specifically, the IGD group (M = 0.33, SE = 0.04) discounted delayed outcomes more quickly than the comparison group did (M = 0.44, SE = 0.04), (F (1,71) = 4.63, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.06). Gains (M = 0.35, SE = 0.03) were discounted more steeply than were losses (M = 0.43, SE = 0.03), (F (1,71) = 6.07, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08). Small amounts (M = 0.32, SE = 0.03) were discounted more steeply than were

Discussion

As of the time of publication of this paper, few studies have investigated the mechanisms underlying IGD in adolescents. Although some studies have investigated delay discounting (Saville et al., 2010; Y. Wang et al., 2017) and even compared the performance on delay discounting and risk-taking (Q. Li et al., 2016, Weinstein et al., 2016) in adults with IGD, to our knowledge, this is the first study to systematically examine two facets of delay discounting and risk-taking in adolescents with

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study is the first to systematically examine the impulsive and risky decision making of adolescents with IGD for gains and losses of large and small amounts. In the delay discounting task, adolescents with IGD had a higher degree of discounting in all conditions, which revealed their general neglect for the future. In contrast, in the probability discounting task, no intergroup difference was found in any condition between IGD group and the comparison group, suggesting that

Author disclosure statement

No competing financial interests exist. The data and manuscript have not been submitted elsewhere in whole or in part. All the authors listed have approved the enclosed manuscript, and they hold themselves jointly and individually responsible for its content.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grants 31200782, 31571161 and 31070987) and China Scholarship Council.

References (91)

  • X. Lin et al.

    Impaired risk evaluation in people with internet gaming disorder: fMRI evidence from a probability discounting task

    Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry

    (2015)
  • S.H. Mitchell et al.

    The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: Outcome size matters for gains but not for losses

    Behavioural Processes

    (2010)
  • M. Pawlikowski et al.

    Excessive Internet gaming and decision making: Do excessive World of Warcraft players have problems in decision making under risky conditions?

    Psychiatry Research

    (2011)
  • B. Reynolds et al.

    Delay and probability discounting as related to different stages of adolescent smoking and non-smoking

    Behavioural Processes

    (2003)
  • B. Reynolds et al.

    Adolescent smokers rate delayed rewards as less certain than adolescent nonsmokers

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2007)
  • D. Romer et al.

    Older versus newer media and the well-being of United States Youth: Results from a national longitudinal panel

    Journal of Adolescent Health

    (2013)
  • K.K. Ryan et al.

    The relationship between impulsivity, risk-taking propensity and nicotine dependence among older adolescent smokers

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2013)
  • L. Steinberg

    A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking

    Developmental Review

    (2008)
  • L. Xu et al.

    Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: From discounting future gains to future losses

    Brain Research

    (2009)
  • Y.-W. Yao et al.

    Impaired decision-making under risk is associated with gaming-specific inhibition deficits among college students with Internet gaming disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2015)
  • G. Ainslie

    Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1975)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)
  • M. Arain et al.

    Maturation of the adolescent brain

    Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment

    (2013)
  • F. Baker et al.

    Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2003)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1988)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Comparison of Beck depression inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients

    Journal of Personality Assessment

    (1996)
  • W.K. Bickel et al.

    The behavioral economics of substance use disorders: Reinforcement pathologies and their repair

    Annual Review of Clinical Psychology

    (2014)
  • A.N. Burge et al.

    Age of gambling initiation and severity of gambling and health problems among older adult problem gamblers

    Psychiatric Services

    (2004)
  • B.J. Casey et al.

    The adolescent brain

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2008)
  • J. Cheng et al.

    Temporal discounting in heroin-dependent patients: No sign effect, weaker magnitude effect, and the relationship with inhibitory control

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2012)
  • C.-Y. Chen et al.

    Brain correlates of response inhibition in Internet gaming disorder

    Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

    (2015)
  • W. Du et al.

    Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards

    Psychological Record

    (2002)
  • X. Du et al.

    Altered structural correlates of impulsivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • S.J. Estle et al.

    Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses

    Memory & Cognition

    (2006)
  • B.J. Everitt et al.

    Drug Addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2016)
  • Q. Feng et al.

    Voxel-level comparison of arterial spin-labeled perfusion magnetic resonance imaging in adolescents with internet gaming addiction

    Behavioral and Brain Functions

    (2013)
  • R. Festl et al.

    Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults

    Addiction

    (2013)
  • G.A. Giovino

    Epidemiology of tobacco use among US adolescents

    Nicotine & Tobacco Research

    (1999)
  • L. Green et al.

    A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2004)
  • N.S. Hinvest et al.

    The effects of real versus hypothetical reward on delay and probability discounting

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2010)
  • J.D. Jentsch et al.

    Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions

    Addiction Reviews

    (2014)
  • M.W. Johnson et al.

    An algorithm for identifying nonsystematic delay-discounting data

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2008)
  • M.W. Johnson et al.

    Moderate drug use and delay discounting: A comparison of heavy, light, and never smokers

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2007)
  • M. Kaess et al.

    Pathological internet use among european adolescents: Psychopathology and self-destructive behaviours

    European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • D. Kahneman et al.

    Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk

    Econometrica

    (1979)
  • Cited by (41)

    • Impulse control differentiates Internet gaming disorder from non-disordered but heavy Internet gaming use: Evidence from multiple behavioral and multimodal neuroimaging data

      2022, Computers in Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      In contrast, the non-disordered but heavy Internet gaming players showed the same level of resting-state functional connectivity within the frontoparietal network as the control-for-non-disordered but heavy Internet gaming players. Combined with the finding that there was a correlation between the frontoparietal network and discounting rate, these results may suggest that as the impairment in the frontoparietal network, individuals with Internet gaming disorder may be insensitive to future severe negative consequences and may need more effort to control over the impulse to play Internet games (Tian et al., 2018). Multivariate pattern recognition analyses have been proven to be more sensitive than conventional univariate analyses in assessing the link between behavioral and neuroimaging variables to predict the presence or absence of brain disorders (Reggente et al., 2018).

    • The relationship between delay discounting and Internet addiction: A systematic review and meta-analysis

      2021, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Compared with k value, AUC produced a larger ES for the DD rate due to the extreme magnitude (see Fig. 3). In this meta-analysis, of all the studies adopting AUC, two employed extreme delays (e.g., 25 or 50 years) to amplify the effect of prolonged delay on DD (Li et al., 2016; Tian et al., 2018). Our result was consistent with that of previous studies (Borges, Kuang, Milhorn, & Yi, 2016; Smith & Hantula, 2008).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    These authors contributed equally.

    View full text