Modeling learning and forgetting processes with the corresponding impacts on human behaviors in infectious disease epidemics

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cie.2018.04.035Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Two mathematics models for forgetting and learning behavior in epidemic are proposed.

  • Protection behavior changes are discussed by the information and emotion.

  • Application ranges of two models are determined by analysis and simulations.

  • Two models are used to restore 2009 Chicago H1N1 prevalence in agent-based model.

Abstract

This article presents two new mathematical models, an information forgetting curve (IFC) model and a memory reception fading and cumulating (MRFC) model, to examine forgetting and learning behaviors of individuals during an infectious disease epidemic. Both models consider how epidemic prevalence and community behavior-change information may affect agent emotions and subsequently influence an individual's behavior changes during an epidemic. The IFC model utilizes a forgetting curve to process epidemic information, and the MRFC model formulates disease information variations using the Itô diffusion process. Sensitivity analysis and simulation comparisons showed that the MRFC model more accurately describes the epidemic with high lethal rate gets high attention. The author also demonstrated that MRFC model has higher sensitivity parameters and is more flexible on wide ranges of infection rates than the IFC model. However, the IFC model is a better suited for widespread, low-risk mortality epidemics, such as seasonal influenza, the infection information and protective behavior have close relationships among the susceptible population. An agent-based simulation model also developed to mimic the epidemic prevalence of the 2009 Chicago H1N1 using public available historical data sets by IFC model.

Introduction

Human disease awareness and related behavior changes during disease epidemics have recently attracted considerable research attention (Polgar, 1962). In order to be more accurately predict a disease epidemic and estimate its potential impacts, however, a comprehensive understanding of information dissemination within human contact networks and the effects of this information on human emotions, awareness, and behavior must increase. Extensive literature and studies have investigated how information affects human behaviors, but minimal research has focused on human memory and forgetting/learning processes related to disease information, or the process in which information may be forgotten and relearned during an epidemic episode. This paper proposes two new mathematical models to investigate the effects of information in disease transmission, including the forgetting and learning phenomenon.

The human brain cannot store an infinite amount of retrieved information. In 1968, Shiffrin and Atkinson (1969) first classified memory as long term and short term. Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, and Conway (1999) defined short-term memory as retainable for a short period of time (usually from 6 to 600 s) but unable to be manipulated; however, they did not detail how long-term or short-term memory relates to people forgetting information. Ebbinghaus (1913) experimentally investigated how the process of forgetting proceeds with influences of time or daily events, hypothesizing that, although a memory series is gradually forgotten, memories that have been learned twice fade more slowly compared to memories that have been learned once. Wingfield and Byrnes (2013) proposed a “forgetting curve” to show the process of memory loss over a period from 20 min to 31 days. A recent paper has indicated that the people learn language also following the forgetting curve (Weltens & Cohen, 1989), and experiments have been conducted to increase understanding of the learning and forgetting phenomenon (Badiru, 1992, Bailey, 1989). In 1985, Brainerd, Kingma, and Howe (1985) concluded that forgetting is governed by various laws and therefore requires unique theoretical assumptions.

Notable learning and forgetting mathematical models have been proposed. In 1976, Carlson and Rowe (1976) introduced the variable regression variable forgetting (VRVF) model, and, in 1990, Elm'Aghraby (1990) proposed the variable regression invariant forgetting (VRIF) model, which corrected errors in previous forgetting models and accommodated a finite horizon. In 1996, Jaber and Bonney (1996) proposed the learn-forget curve model (LFCM), which showed that forgetting is dependent on some factors such as the learning slope, the quantity produced and the minimum production breaks. In 1997, Jaber and Bonney (1997) compared these three models, and in recent years, Jaber, Kher, and Davis (2003) reviewed factors that influence forgetting and incorporated the job similarity factor into the LFCM. In 2002, Sikström and Jaber (2002) provided an elaborate review of forgetting curves in psychology and industrial engineering literature. Because the previous papers did not consider the forgetting phenomenon in disease, however, this paper proposes an information forgetting curve model (IFC) to describe how disease information fades over time during an epidemic, following a forgetting curve through time and thereby influencing final disease memory. In 2012, Sikström & Jaber updated their research in the modeling of learning and forgetting area (Sikström & Jaber, 2012). They proposed a Depletion-Power-Integration-Latency (DPIL) Model. This model considered the depletion of the encoding resource as forgetting and learning behavior when the system repetitively performs a task. More than fitting the historical dataset and calculate the settings of optimal performance, this model discussed how learning can interact with the forgetting by modeling the repetitively encoding can increase the memory strength.

Stochastic factors can influence the information perception process and new information can diversely affect human memory when agents receive new information and forget previous information. Researchers (Slovic, 2016) have shown that biased media coverage, misleading personal experiences, and anxieties can cause people to process information with unwarranted confidence or uncertain judgment. Zhao, Wu, Kuang, Bi, and Ben-Arieh (2018) also discussed the stochastic change rate of perception to infectious disease risk. This paper proposes a memory reception fading and cumulating (MRFC) model to describe the stochastic phenomenon of human memory as it pertains to disease information based on learning and forgetting.

Agents have unique understandings based on identical information, and they react with distinct switch behaviors. In 2009, Chen (2009) concluded that agents learn prevalence through the spread of information and can adjust human behavior during a disease epidemic. Funk, Gilad, Watkins, and Jansen (2009) found that the spread of disease awareness significantly decreases the infection rate, and Kiss, Cassell, Recker, and Simon (2010) proved that the diffusion of disease information increases risk awareness and causes the host population to take infection prevention measures. In 2015, Zhao, Wu, Kuang, and Ben-Arieh (2015) proposed a disease model using a spatial evolutionary game to illustrate the impact of information dissemination on human behavior in an epidemic, proving that how an agent feels depends on information content and context (Nahl & Bilal, 2007). In other words, agents demonstrate unique perspectives for the same disease information, resulting in diverse emotional responses. When disease information is positive, agents may have minimal concerns about the disease; negative information, however, may increase agent's awareness. Hence, certain types of information could alter agents’ moods or emotions (Pessoa, 2008). Chen, Bi, Zhao, Ben-Arieh, and Wu (2017) also modeled how disease information, such as the number of infected individuals and the number of susceptible individuals who choose the switching behaviors, impact agents' fears about the epidemic. This paper applies an agent-based model to determine how disease information can cause diverse human behaviors, as well as use of the one-factor-at-a-time method (OFAT) to conduct sensitivity analysis for various parameter settings in the IFC and MRFC models, highlighting the influence of parameter settings for the model and comparing agents’ epidemic behaviors using the IFC, MRFC, and no-memory models.

This paper also discusses the 2009 H1N1 influenza epidemic. By combining historical infection data in affected cities with corresponding population characteristics, the authors restored the 2009 H1N1 prevalence in Chicago, a typical H1N1-affected city. This paper also investigates how the phenomenon of memory fading and behavior-switched protection influence epidemic spreading.

Section snippets

Contact network and disease information

Contact networks have been widely applied to many implementations of disease transmission (Altizer, 2003, Bansal et al., 2007, Lloyd-Smith et al., 2005). Zhao et al. (2015) introduced the concept of disease information dissemination and its effect on epidemic disease transmission by proposing that agents gain disease information from two layers: local and global contact networks. Local information is gained from neighboring agents, while global information is acquired from all locations. The

Memory reception and fading

Based on the study by Shiffrin and Atkinson (1969), the memory fading process occurs at the same time as the information reception process. The main objective of this section is to establish the memory fading model for received disease information. Similar to Eq. (2.4), gained information γi(t) of agent i over time period t can be divided into infected information and surrounding switching behavior information collected on the ITCN and DTCN, γi(t) represents all information transmitted from the

Agent-based modeling

This section discusses agent-based modeling simulation of epidemic transmission to determine if IFC and MRFC models are effective in the real world. Because individuals have unique memories, moods, and behaviors, the unit of simulation must be individual, hence the use of the agent-based model. Agents are typically categorized into four types: switch susceptible, normal susceptible, infected, and recovery. An infected agent can contaminate a nearby switch/normal susceptible agent with a varying

H1N1 pandemic in 2009

H1N1, also known as swine flu (Peiris, Poon, & Guan, 2009), is a form of influenza in pigs that can be transmitted to people by exposure to infected droplets. H1N1 is an orthomyxovirus, a subtype of influenza A that is the most common cause of seasonal human flu. H1N1 viruses attack the human immune system, attaching and replicating within infected cells. A person infected by the H1N1 virus will develop a progressive lower respiratory tract disease that could result in respiratory failure (

Summary

This paper investigated the assumption that disease information can influence an individual's fear emotion and that agents’ emotions potentially affect behavior during an epidemic. This study used two mathematical models (IFC and MRFC models) to discuss disease information fading and learning processes. Both models synthesized disease information on local and global levels with infection information and switched-behavior information, thereby providing comprehensive disease information to

References (71)

  • M.A. Pires et al.

    Dynamics of epidemic spreading with vaccination: Impact of social pressure and engagement

    Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications

    (2017)
  • E. Plennevaux et al.

    Immune response after a single vaccination against 2009 influenza A H1N1 in USA: A preliminary report of two randomised controlled phase 2 trials

    The Lancet

    (2010)
  • N. Ruiz-Rivera et al.

    Urban segregation and local retail environments. Evidence from Mexico City

    Habitat International

    (2016)
  • S. Sikström et al.

    The Depletion–Power–Integration–Latency (DPIL) model of spaced and massed repetition

    Computers & Industrial Engineering

    (2012)
  • Pauline Van den Driessche et al.

    Reproduction numbers and sub-threshold endemic equilibria for compartmental models of disease transmission.

    Mathematical Biosciences

    (2002)
  • M.A. Wakefield et al.

    Use of mass media campaigns to change health behaviour

    The Lancet

    (2010)
  • Sonia Altizer

    Social organization and parasite risk in mammals: Integrating theory and empirical studies

    Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics

    (2003)
  • L.G. Alvarez-Zuzek et al.

    Epidemic spreading in multiplex networks influenced by opinion exchanges on vaccination

    PLoS ONE

    (2017)
  • R.B. Anderson et al.

    Artifactual power curves in forgetting

    Memory & Cognition

    (1997)
  • Archer, B. N., Cohen, C., Naidoo, D., Thomas, J., Makunga, C., Blumberg, L. H., … Schoub, B. D. (2009). Interim report...
  • Alan D. Baddeley

    Human memory: Theory and practice

    (1997)
  • A.B. Badiru

    Computational survey of univariate and multivariate learning curve models

    IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management

    (1992)
  • F. Bagnoli et al.

    Risk perception in epidemic modeling

    Physical Review E

    (2007)
  • C.D. Bailey

    Forgetting and the learning curve: A laboratory study

    Management Science

    (1989)
  • S. Bansal et al.

    When individual behaviour matters: Homogeneous and network models in epidemiology

    Journal of the Royal Society Interface

    (2007)
  • N.G. Becker
    (1989)
  • C.J. Brainerd et al.

    On the development of forgetting

    Child Development

    (1985)
  • Carolyn Buxton Bridges

    Effectiveness and cost-benefit of influenza vaccination of healthy working adults: A randomized controlled trial

    JAMA

    (2000)
  • J.G. Carlson et al.

    How much does forgetting cost

    Industrial Engineering

    (1976)
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2009. Interim guidance on infection control measures for 2009 H1N1...
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2009. Pandemic Influenza A (H1N1) virus infections – Chicago, Illinois....
  • K.W. Chan

    Misconceptions and complexities in the study of China's cities: Definitions, statistics, and implications

    Eurasian Geography and Economics

    (2007)
  • H. Ebbinghaus

    Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology

    (1913)
  • S.E. Elm'Aghraby

    Economic manufacturing quantities under conditions of learning and forgetting (EMQ/LaF)

    Production Planning & Control

    (1990)
  • R.W. Engle et al.

    Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1999)
  • Cited by (22)

    • An optimal control policy in fighting COVID-19 and infectious diseases

      2022, Applied Soft Computing
      Citation Excerpt :

      It could be because of the type of the job that a person has, like social workers, medical personnel and cab drivers, but here in this study we are considering the impact of fear sentiment. Behavior of individuals heavily depends on the amount of fear sentiment which they receive [12]. Face masks combined with other preventive measures, such as self-quarantine and frequent hand-washing, help slow the spread of the virus [41].

    • Research on adaptive transmission and controls of COVID-19 on the basis of a complex network

      2021, Computers and Industrial Engineering
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, contact and travelling are the essential factors that induce COVID-19 transmission. Along with the contagion of panic from the infected high-risk places, the induced or mandatory minimal contact and travelling in these places lead to dynamic differentiated transmission relative to other places (Bi et al., 2019). The mandatory lockdowns in all communities or high-risk places are feasible and effective measures to control COVID-19.

    • The spreading of infectious diseases with recurrent mobility of community population

      2020, Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications
      Citation Excerpt :

      The mobility of human and collective behaviors affect the propagation of diseases [5–7]. The specific locations and recurrent mobility patterns often dominate human interactions [8–12]. The study of epidemic spreading in co-evolving, time-varying and multiscale mobility networks has become a hot topic in complex networks [13–19].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text