Elsevier

Computers & Education

Volume 55, Issue 2, September 2010, Pages 892-903
Computers & Education

The benefits of more electronic screen space on students’ retention of material in classroom lectures

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2010.03.020Get rights and content

Abstract

Many lecture halls today have two or more screens to be used by instructors for lectures with computer-supported visual aids. Typically, this additional screen real estate is not used to display additional information; rather a single stream of information is projected on all screens. We describe a controlled laboratory study that empirically assesses the effect on students learning of using the increased classroom screen real estate to project an additional stream of information. We measured how well participants learned from a two-stream presentation compared to a one-stream presentation duplicated on both screens. Data indicate that using extra screen real estate can indeed improve learning. In particular, learning was most improved when pertinent prior information was shown alongside currently explained information. There is also evidence that visual comparisons were improved with parallel viewing using extra screen real estate. Subjective data gathered from participants showed a strong preference for learning with two streams of content over a regular one-stream presentation.

Introduction

For many decades lectures were presented in classrooms with multiple blackboards, providing a large amount of “screen real estate” on which material could persist. With the advent of personal computers, commercial software such as PowerPoint, and digital projectors, many lectures now use computer-supported slides as their primary visual aids. Most studies that examined if computer-generated slides are beneficial in classroom lectures have found that students responded positively to the use of computer-generated slides when compared to blackboards or overhead transparencies (Bartsch and Cobern, 2003, Daniels, 1999, Savoy et al., 2009, Susskind, 2005, Susskind, 2008, Szabo and Hastings, 2000). Students indicated that slides help them improve organization of the course and their notes, help them learn course material more effectively, and make lectures more interesting and entertaining (Levasseur & Sawyer, 2006). How students react to computer slides is important, but an equally important question is whether computer slides actually influence student learning. Most studies examining the effect computer slides have on learning outcomes have found no significant improvement in student performance when slides were used, compared to using other visual aids such as overhead transparencies or blackboards (Bartsch and Cobern, 2003, Daniels, 1999, Savoy et al., 2009, Susskind, 2005, Susskind, 2008, Szabo and Hastings, 2000).

One reason that electronic slides might not be as effective as traditional visual aids is the lower screen resolution that PowerPoint lectures have compared to a blackboard lecture. Using PowerPoint, instructors typically use one medium-resolution projector screen to show their slides. This is not comparable to the resolution provided by a typical blackboard lecture (Lanir, Booth, & Findlater, 2008). A single medium-resolution screen limits the amount of information that can be presented, making it difficult to explain complex, non-linear concepts (Tufte, 2003). The restricted display area of slides may also cause authors to edit their content and eliminate details in order to fit onto slides, or to reorganize content onto two or more slides in a way that breaks the logical organization of the material being presented (Farkas, 2009).

As universities accommodate more students and face increasing financial and resource constraints, many classes, especially at the introductory level, are taught in large lecture halls. While most educators agree that smaller classes provide better learning environments with stronger student–instructor interaction, large lectures are likely to persist in the near future. Fortunately, the same economies of scale that have led to large class sizes and large lecture halls also allow universities to invest in technological infrastructure for lecture facilities (Wolfman, 2002). We thus find that many large lecture halls are now equipped with multiple projector screens. With increasing resolution on large displays, increasing computer power to support multiple displays and animation, and decreasing projector prices, it is likely that future lecture halls will have even more screen real estate using higher resolution projectors or more projectors. Lately, different software tools have been developed to make use of these larger electronic surfaces (Chiu et al., 2003, Lanir et al., 2008, Röüling et al., 2004).

We are interested in determining whether the more limited screen real estate usually available for computer-supported presentations compared to traditional blackboards has perhaps been detrimental to students’ learning. More positively, we want to know whether using additional screen real estate for computer-supported presentations might improve learning. Using more screen resolution to present more information and aid teaching is most evident when instructors use multiple sliding blackboards (we use the term “blackboard” to refer generically to either a traditional blackboard with chalk or to a whiteboard with felt markers). Multiple sliding blackboards in large lecture halls have been used for decades in many university courses. Lanir, Booth, et al. (2008) observed how instructors use a wide array of blackboards and compared that with the use of computer slides in classrooms. They found that many techniques easily used with blackboards have been made more difficult or lost with the move to computer-generated presentations. We summarize these findings: (1) A blackboard allows instructors to present ideas using a large surface that they can dynamically control: they can decide which information to erase and which to keep for future reference. (2) Multiple blackboards allow large amounts of information to be visible to students, which computer “slideware” systems seldom do. (3) Larger surfaces, such as multiple blackboards, are useful for supporting explanations of complex ideas. With more space, spatial relationship between concepts can be used to encode meaning. (4) Instructors use blackboards to show relationships between two concepts drawn on different boards, or to compare those items. (5) Because blackboards afford more screen space than computer slides, information can persist for a longer time. With blackboards, instructors would often refer back to previously written content during a lecture – sometimes to information recently presented, and other times to content originating much earlier in the presentation. With computers slides, this rarely happened.

In this paper we report a controlled study that considers the efficacy of using more screen space in electronic classroom presentations. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have thus far examined the advantages and tradeoffs of using extra electronic space. Our study was conducted in a classroom with two screens available for projection. We compared a “one-stream presentation” (i.e., a normal PowerPoint presentation with slides duplicated on each screen) with a “two-stream presentation” in which the additional screen real estate was used to present slide content that was different from the slides projected in the primary stream of information. The study evaluated whether students learned better when presented with information in two-streams on the two projected surfaces compared to a regular, one-stream presentation duplicated on both surfaces. In particular, we examined different practices for presenting the additional information in the second stream to see if they have an effect on students’ learning.

Section snippets

Study objectives

The primary objective of the study was to more thoroughly examine the relationship between learning and using additional screen real estate in a typical classroom presentation scenario. A secondary objective was to drill down to examine in what cases providing a secondary stream of information improves learning, in which cases it might not help learning, and whether there are times it actually hinders learning. We designed a controlled laboratory study that simulated a classroom lecture with

Methodology

The experiment was a 2 × 2 × 2 mixed design with delivery style (one-stream or two-stream) and lecture topic (human visual system vs. human auditory system) as within-subject variables and lecture order as a between-subject control variable. Similar to the experimental design of Susskind, 2005, Susskind, 2008, participants did not see each lecture in both conditions, but rather saw one lecture topic with each delivery style condition. A within-subject design was chosen to enable us to gather

Hypotheses

Based on our research objectives, we established the following five experimental hypotheses:

  • H1. Information displayed using two streams of slides can improve learning over a one-stream presentation.

  • H2. Showing pertinent information from previously shown slides on a secondary screen improves the retention of information.

  • H3. Increasing the persistency of information by showing it for a longer time (such as showing the previous slide as well as the current slide) improves the retention of

Results

We first present our analysis of participants’ previous knowledge of the lecture topics. We then present the results for the multiple-choice quiz administered during the first lecture session (immediate retention). We continue with the results for the questions administered a week later (deferred retention), after which we present the results for the subjective questionnaires.

Discussion

Our results provide encouraging evidence that using multiple screens in classroom presentations can improve learning. On the overall retention scores obtained immediately after a lecture, participants performed significantly better in the two-stream condition than in the one-stream condition. However, this general effect did not carry over a week later when participants were again tested. This may be because of higher variability and lower scores in this test. Drilling down through the

Conclusions

We described a controlled laboratory study to formally evaluate the advantages of two-stream presentations over one-stream presentations. The results indicate that, when used properly, a two-stream lecture can improve learning over a regular one-stream lecture. In particular, the two-stream presentation style was most useful when pertinent prior information was shown on the secondary screen alongside the current information. The study also provides some support for the benefits of using two

References (19)

  • P. Ayres et al.

    The split-attention principle in multimedia learning

  • R.A. Bartsch et al.

    Effectiveness of power point presentations in lectures

    Computers & Education

    (2003)
  • B.S. Bloom et al.

    Taxonomy of educational objectives longmans

    (1956)
  • J. Bransford

    How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school

    (2000)
  • P. Chiu et al.

    Manipulating and annotating slides in a multi-display environment

    Proceedings of INTERACT’03

    (2003)
  • L. Daniels

    Introducing technology in the classroom: powerpoint as a first step

    Journal of Computing in Higher Education

    (1999)
  • D.K. Farkas

    Managing three mediation effects that influence powerpoint deck authoring

    Technical Communication

    (2009)
  • S.W. Huck et al.

    Reading statistics and research

    (1974)
  • J. Lanir et al.

    Observing presenters’ use of visual aids to inform the design of classroom presentation software

    Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems

    (2008)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (9)

  • Re-mediating classroom activity with a non-linear, multi-display presentation tool

    2013, Computers and Education
    Citation Excerpt :

    For example, the MultiPresenter tool (Lanir et al., 2010) was used in an empirical study comparing ‘two-stream’ and ‘one-stream’ lecture presentations, which found significantly better student information retention from two-stream presentations under two conditions: when one information stream provided long-term context for the other, or when the two streams contained information for direct comparison. Within the literature, evaluations of novel presentation tools can be categorised as technical descriptions (Rößling et al., 2004), case studies of enthusiasts' use with their own students (Bligh and Lorenz, 2010), or quasi-experimental studies where the behaviour of tool users is narrowly specified by the terms of the experiment (Lanir et al., 2010). What is absent is any detailed evaluation of the ways these novel tools interact with authentic users' established presentation activity in naturalistic teaching and learning settings, or more specifically how novel presentation tools re-mediate existing activity systems.

  • Slide presentations as speech suppressors: When and why learners miss oral information

    2012, Computers and Education
    Citation Excerpt :

    At this level of generality, the driving question of many of these studies seems to be overly general, and more specific research questions seem to be appropriate. Fortunately, some of the studies already discussed also focus on limiting conditions for the effectiveness of using presentation software in teaching, for example in courses differing according to the kinds of their topics (Kunkel, 2004, p. 192), with learners differing according to learning prerequisites such as “learning styles” (Nouri & Shahid, 2005, pp. 62–65), “cognitive styles” (Carrell & Menzel, 2001, study 1, p. 236; study 2, p. 238) or learners’ preferred modes of presentation (Beets & Lobingier, 2001, p. 234; for an overview of these aptitude–treatment interaction studies see Levasseur & Sawyer, 2006, p. 113 f.), with slideshows differing according to their multimedia design aspects (Bartsch & Cobern, 2003, study 1, p. 81; study 2, p. 83; Hallett & Faria, 2006, p. 173 f.), or with tests tapping different kinds or levels of knowledge (Stephenson, Brown, & Griffin, 2008, p. 646 f.) or retention of information differentiated according to the mode in which it was presented (e. g. graphically or orally; Lanir, Booth, & Hawkey, 2010, p. 897; Savoy, Proctor, & Salvendy, 2009, p. 862). One of these studies with a more specific focus demonstrated a negative effect of the use of PowerPoint on the recall of information that was presented only orally (Savoy et al., 2009, p. 863), which is in line with the occasional impression mentioned above.

  • An Examination of the Personal Technology of Students in STEM Disciplines

    2022, Proceedings - Frontiers in Education Conference, FIE
  • Orchestrating multi-device presentations with omnipresent

    2017, PerDis 2017 - Proceedings: 6th ACM International Symposium on Pervasive Displays
  • The use of a multi-display system in university classroom lectures

    2016, Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Conference on Interactive Surfaces and Spaces: Nature Meets Interactive Surfaces, ISS 2016
View all citing articles on Scopus
1

Tel.: +604 8223061; fax: +604 8225485.

View full text