Elsevier

Computers & Education

Volume 69, November 2013, Pages 31-37
Computers & Education

Rotational perspective and learning procedural tasks from dynamic media

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.06.014Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Previous research has found a differential effect of animations on learning.

  • Some suggest animations of procedural tasks activate the mirror neuron system.

  • If true, animations which lessen such activation should lead to less learning.

  • Animations most likely to activate mirror neurons produced the best learning.

  • Animations that should lessen mirror neuron activation are less effective.

Abstract

There have been conflicting accounts regarding the effectiveness of animations for learning. Procedural motor learning represents one of the few areas in which animations have consistently shown to be facilitative. Some have suggested that this benefit is related to activation of the mirror neuron system (MNS), with higher activation leading to better performance. This study examines this explanation, and observed the effects of instructional media (animation vs. static), as a function of viewing perspective (face-to-face vs. over-the-shoulder) on understanding a procedural motor task (knot tying). Results indicate that performance was significantly improved with animations over static images, however this appeared to be most pronounced in situations which matched the learners' own perspective (i.e., over-the-shoulder). These findings have implications for the design of instructional media for procedural motor tasks and provide confirmation of the assertion that appropriate activation of the perceptual system can be leveraged to increase performance.

Introduction

The notion that dynamic external visualizations can provide some efficacy over alternative presentations such as textual descriptions has been suggested many times (Larkin and Simon, 1987, Tufte, 2001). While there is some evidence suggesting that animations do not facilitate learning (Boucheix and Schneider, 2009, ChanLin, 2000, Hegarty et al., 2003, Mayer et al., 2005, Tversky et al., 2002), numerous other studies have found that animations can indeed enhance learning under certain constraints (see Höffler & Leutner (2007) for a review). For example, animations have been shown to be more effective than static visualizations for the acquisition of process knowledge in cell biology (Münzer, Seufert, & Brünken, 2009), and learning concepts in chemistry and earth science (Falvo and Suits, 2009, Sanchez and Wiley, 2010). However, an open question is what leads to this differential pattern of results regarding the effectiveness of animations for learning?

Some have suggested that perhaps animations are particularly well suited for certain content areas, and less so for others. In a meta-analysis conducted by Höffler and Leutner (2007), it was found that animations can be more effective than static images if they are a realistic approximation of the task, and especially so if the task involves procedural motor learning. For example, studies on the learning of basic first aid procedures (e.g., what to do when someone is choking, or how to bandage a hand) found that animations produced higher levels of recall than static images or text alone, and also produced faster rates of learning (Arguel and Jamet, 2009, Michas and Berry, 2000). Similarly, Ayres, Marcus, Chan, and Qian (2009) found that observing animations of knot tying and ring assembly puzzles produced the highest levels of both forward and reverse learning, and increased the ability to recognize next or previous steps versus similar static presentations.

So what is it about procedural motor learning tasks that make them so amenable to animated presentations? As animations demonstrate changes over time and/or sequence, it is possible that the inclusion of this underlying temporal component might produce a higher degree of match to procedural tasks versus other contexts, as these procedural tasks themselves often contain a temporal sequence of events. This is consistent with the intuitions of Rieber & Kini (1991), who suggested that animations can reduce the abstract nature of temporal changes, thus providing a more tangible way to understand material. In other words, the representation of time in these dynamic visualizations potentially increases the match between the to-be-learned material, and how said information is instructionally conveyed. Simply, the method of instruction more closely matches the constraints of the content domain it is meant to represent. It has been speculated that this congruence between content area and instruction underlies the most effective animations (Rieber, 1990; Tversky et al., 2002).

How then are we able to capitalize on heightened levels of congruence between instruction and content to improve learning and thus realize the full benefit of animations? In other words, what might be the underlying mechanism by which the effects of congruence are realized? While to date this has not been explicitly tested neurologically, it has been proposed that such animations potentially activate the mirror neuron system (MNS). It is believed that this activation facilitates learning in these tasks by providing a perceptual basis to interpret and understand these visualizations (Ayres et al., 2009, Chandler, 2009, Wong et al., 2009), akin to the perceptual benefit originally attributed to static illustrations alone (Larkin & Simon, 1987). From this perspective, activation of the MNS permits an almost effortless simulation or embodiment of the material which can then be used to encourage the activation of relevant knowledge and connections between material, and thus better understanding (van Gog, Paas, Marcus, Ayres & Sweller, 2009).

Originally identified in primates, the MNS represents a neurophysiological circuit distributed across the pre-motor cortex that selectively activates when either conducting a specific action, or when observing another individual performing the same action (Pellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese & Rizzolatti, 1992). It was originally believed that this system offered a perceptual mechanism to understand goal-directed action and intent (Gallese & Goldman, 1998) and perhaps might even serve as a gateway to language and communication (Gallese et al., 1996, Rizzolatti and Arbib, 1998). More recent research has refined this definition and identifies the MNS as critical for not only understanding others' movements, but also generating plans for one's own actions and execution (Fogassi et al., 2005, Meltzoff and Prinz, 2002). For example, while simply observing an individual play guitar chords did activate the MNS, watching the same material with the intent to replicate or reproduce (i.e., to learn) the actions produced even higher levels of activation within the MNS (Buccino et al., 2004). As such, some have suggested that this underlying architecture is potentially responsible for our ability to learn through imitation, which is a unique to higher apes and humans (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). Given this conceptualization, it is not surprising that this neural circuit might be especially relevant for procedural learning, as the fundamental goal of any procedural learning task is to successfully imitate or reproduce a given pattern of motor events.

If this mirror neuron explanation is correct, and thus mediates the effectiveness of a given animation, one would expect to see an interaction between animations and the degree to which they activate the MNS. For example, when animations most activate the MNS, we would expect to see the highest levels of learning as these visualizations leverage the perceptual system in such a way as to enable better understanding of the to-be-learned motor task. However, when animations fail to strongly activate the MNS, it is reasonable to expect a similar decline in overall performance as this perceptual processing benefit is not realized. One way to test this explanation would be to systematically vary the degree of activation of the MNS, while holding informational content constant. Shifting action away from first-person perspectives has been shown to moderate activation of the MNS, such that face-to-face presentations activate the MNS to a lesser degree than over-the-shoulder perspectives (Jackson et al., 2006, Maeda et al., 2002). In other words, watching someone else demonstrate an action has been shown to produce lower levels of activation in the MNS than watching action from a perspective more akin to actual experience (e.g., through your own eyes). The question then is: does reducing activation of the MNS simultaneously produce lower levels of learning from animations when informational content is the same? If the suggestion that the MNS is largely responsible for the facilitation of animations in procedural learning, than one would expect lower facilitation in situations that less strongly activate the MNS.

Finally, previous research has indicated that individual differences in visuospatial and attentional ability might also moderate the instructional effectiveness of any kind of visualization, including animations. Visuospatial ability has been shown to interact with animations to affect learning of mechanical information (Mayer, 2001; Mayer & Sims, 1994), and spatial science topics (Höffler and Leutner, 2011, Sanchez and Wiley, 2010, Yang et al., 2003). Similarly, Cognitive Load Theory (van Merriënboer & Sweller, 2005), Mayer's (2005) Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning, and Schnotz's (2005) Integrative Model of Text and Picture Comprehension all speculate that appropriate visualizations offload processing demand from the working memory system, thereby freeing resources to better dedicate to the learning process. Given these proposed connections between learning from visualizations and individual differences in cognitive ability, it seems prudent to control for these abilities when evaluating a causal theory such as the MNS explanation.

To explore learning in procedural motor tasks and how they may interact with our perceptual system and presentation, a study was conducted which manipulated both of these characteristics. Participants were shown a procedural knot tying task from either an animated or static media, which was either consistent with their perspective when completing the task themselves (i.e., over-the-shoulder, OTS), or instead rotated 180° into a face-to-face (FTF) orientation. It is hypothesized that for procedural motor learning tasks, instruction via animations instead of static images will facilitate overall performance resulting in faster time, fewer errors and more steps correct. This would be consistent with a host of psychological research regarding the efficacy of such dynamic media (Höffler & Leutner, 2007). Further, this performance benefit may also interact with perspective of the demonstration. If the mirror neuron explanation is correct, those orientations that would activate the MNS most strongly will realize the largest benefits of animation. Conversely, those that lessen activation of the MNS should perform less well. In order to control for individual differences in general cognitive processing and visuospatial manipulation, both of which have been suggested to influence learning from animations (Hegarty and Waller, 2005, Mayer and Moreno, 1998), additional measures of these cognitive abilities were collected and entered into subsequent analyses.

Section snippets

Participants

Eighty-six undergraduates from a large public university were solicited for participation in this experiment. To control for differences in prior knowledge, participants were asked to self-report any formal prior experience with knot tying before participation. Participants who reported any formal familiarity with knot tying were excluded from the final analysis. Based on this strict criteria, 6 participants were excluded from the final analysis, resulting in eighty participants overall. All

Results & discussion

Participant's task performance was measured in the following ways: total time on task, and proportion of correct attempts. Descriptive statistics are available in Table 1. To test whether the rotation of the media and instructional media differed, a hierarchical OLS regression was performed for each of these performance metrics. Type of Media, perspective, WMC and visuospatial ability measures were entered into the first block of the analysis, and then an interaction term between media and

Conclusions

This experiment was designed to test two previous assertions regarding the potential benefit of animations in educational contexts: do animations enhance performance in procedural motor tasks, and more importantly, can such increases be directly tied to activation of the perceptual MNS? While animations did lead to better performance over static images for both the time required to reach a solution, and also the number of correct steps learned, it appears that these effects were largest for

References (45)

  • G. Rizzolatti et al.

    Language within our grasp

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (1998)
  • C.A. Sanchez et al.

    Sex differences in science learning: closing the gap through animations

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2010)
  • A. Wong et al.

    Instructional animations can be superior to statics when learning human motor skills

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2009)
  • G. Budworth

    Useful knots

    (2004)
  • L.J. ChanLin

    Attributes of animation for learning scientific knowledge

    Journal of Instructional Psychology

    (2000)
  • A.R.A. Conway et al.

    Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2005)
  • M. Driscoll

    Web-based training: Creating e-learning experiences

    (2010)
  • D.A. Falvo et al.

    Gender and spatial ability and the use of specific labels and diagrammatic arrows in a micro-level chemistry animation

    Journal of Educational Computing Research

    (2009)
  • L. Fogassi et al.

    Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding

    Science

    (2005)
  • J.W. French et al.

    Kit of reference tests for cognitive factors

    (1963)
  • V. Gallese et al.

    Action recognition in the premotor cortex

    Brain

    (1996)
  • T. van Gog et al.

    The mirror neuron system and observational learning: Implications for the effectiveness of dynamic visualizations

    Educational Psychology Review

    (2009)
  • Cited by (39)

    • Investigating gender and spatial measurements in instructional animation research

      2018, Computers in Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Researchers believe that animations have a greater flexibility in depicting physical and temporal changes than static pictures (Bétrancourt & Tversky, 2000; Castro-Alonso, Ayres, & Paas, 2014a, p. 552; Lowe, 2003), and are often assumed to be superior to static pictures (Chandler, 2009). There is supporting evidence showing that animations are more effective than static pictures when teaching statistics (e.g., Wender & Muehlboeck, 2003), physics (e.g., Rebetez, Bétrancourt, Sangin, & Dillenbourg, 2010; Rieber, 1990, 1991b), geometry (e.g., Thompson & Riding, 1990), zoology (e.g., Pfeiffer, Scheiter, Kühl, & Gemballa, 2011), computer algorithm (e.g., Byrne, Catrambone, & Stasko, 1999), and motor-related tasks (e.g., Akinlofa, Holt, & Elyan, 2013; Ayres, Marcus, Chan, & Qian, 2009; Castro-Alonso, Ayres, & Paas, 2015a; Garland & Sánchez, 2013; Michas & Berry, 2000; A.; Wong et al., 2009). However, some contradictory results have emerged that challenge the effectiveness of animations (e.g., Bétrancourt, Dillenbourg, & Clavien, 2008; Höffler & Leutner, 2007).

    • Mixed camera viewpoints improve learning medical hand procedure from video in nurse training?

      2018, Computers in Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, Jackson, Meltzoff, and Decety (2006) compared the two viewpoints and found more activity in the left sensory-motor cortex for the first-person than for the third-person perspective (see also, Maeda, Kleiner-Fisman & Pascual-Leone, 2002; Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2008). Taking the hypothesis of the modulation of activation of the mirror neuron system, Garland and Sanchez (2013) tested the effect of different viewpoints on university students learning to tie complex knots, comparing dynamic and static presentations with face-to-face (FtF) and over-the-shoulder (OtS) views. Results revealed that learning performance was significantly better with animations than static images, and that this improvement was most pronounced in the situation which matched the learners' own perspective, in other words, the OtS view.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text