Decision Support
The SEXTANT software: A tool for automating the comparative analysis of mental models of dynamic systems

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejor.2014.04.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Most management situations are dynamic; and manager’s decisions are framed by their mental models.

  • Mental models of dynamic systems (MMDS) have three levels of representation: elements, loops and the whole model.

  • Distance ratios at each level reveal differences both between subjects and over time.

  • SEXTANT calculates the distance ratios for elements, loops and the whole model for large samples of MMDS.

  • SEXTANT enables researchers to strongly reduce the efforts to apply MMDS research.

Abstract

The comparison of mental models of dynamic systems improves our understanding of how people comprehend, interpret, and subsequently influence dynamic management tasks. Approaches to comparing mental models currently used in managerial and organizational cognition research, especially the distance-ratio and the closeness-approaches, have been criticized for not considering essential characteristics of dynamic managerial situations. This paper builds on a recent analysis method developed to compare mental models of dynamics systems, and introduces this mathematical approach to management and organizational researchers by means of the SEXTANT software. It presents the process of mental model elicitation, analysis, comparison, and interpretation. An example with four elicited mental models illustrates the software’s features to analyze and present the results. Then, the software is compared with existing software to map and compare mental models. Our conclusion is that SEXTANT marks a significant step in enabling large-scale studies about mental models of dynamic systems.

Introduction

Managers have to make purposeful decisions and implement actions in dynamic environments. Scholars in the field of managerial and organizational cognition (MOC) have studied how managers make sense of influence, causality, and systems’ dynamics in such environments (e.g., Eden et al., 1992, Hodgkinson, 2002, Huff, 1990). Perception and the mental process resulting in decisions are framed by mental models—an internal representation of conceptual and causal interrelations among elements that people use to understand phenomena (Groesser, 2012). In general, such models are represented by some form of conceptual diagram or cause map.

Cause mapping is an approach to representing managers’ mental models about a decision situation which has been used, for instance, to study the formation of beliefs (Markoczy, 1997), strategic change (Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008, Markoczy, 2000, Markoczy, 2001), top management team decisions (Ensley & Pearce, 2001), and team performance (Ellis, 2006, Mohammed and Dumville, 2001, Osborne et al., 2001). Research about managerial and organizational cognition has also shown that more accurate mental models can lead to more successful and more sustainable decisions, especially since a dynamic approach to decision making has the potential to make significant contributions to how managers adopt strategies and how well they perform (e.g., Gary and Wood, 2011, Kunc and Morecroft, 2010, Paich and Sterman, 1993, Sterman, 2010).

One of the latest developments in managerial and organizational cognition is the notion of mental models of dynamic systems (MMDS). Besides other common approaches to representing knowledge (e.g., Bougon, 1992, Brown, 1992, Clarke and Mackaness, 2001, Hodgkinson, 2002, Huff, 1990, Yukso and Goldstein, 2004), MMDS are useful for conceptualizing how humans think, reason, make sense, and learn about systems’ dynamics, that is, systems that change in response to decisions taken to influence them (Doyle, 1997, Doyle and Ford, 1999, Groesser, 2012, Groesser and Schaffernicht, 2012).

Once MMDS have been elicited, they can be compared. There are two purposes for MMDS comparison. First, pre- and post-comparison, i.e., differences between MMDS of individuals at several instances of time, allows one to estimate the learning of individuals over time (Schaffernicht, 2006, Sterman, 2010). And second, computing differences between several individual MMDS at the same point in time, for instance between novices and experts, allows one to assess the current level of individuals’ comprehension of dynamic situations as compared to a reference model which typically represents the understanding of an expert.

Groesser and Schaffernicht (2012) operationalize MMDS on three levels: elements, which contain variables and causal links, then individual feedback loops, and finally the complete model. This operational definition allows comparing MMDS to gain insights about the degree of similarity of such models. With this, research about MMDS can advance to levels which research about mental model of static systems has already achieved (Fiol and Huff, 1992, Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008, Tyler and Gnyawali, 2009). Since MMDS specifically address dynamic situations by means of conceptual components which traditional mental models do not take into account, a specific method for comparison has been developed by Schaffernicht and Groesser (2011). They define the levels of comparison as the Element Distance Ratio (EDR), a set of Loop Distance Ratios (LDRs), and a Model Distance Ratio (MDR). The measure of the dissimilarity of two MMDS is the distance between both. Existing mental model approaches and software tools do not operationalize the MMDS approach.2

The paper advances MMDS research by introducing and applying a sequence of steps to be carried out in MMDS comparison along with the SEXTANT software tool—a set of algorithms which automates most of these steps. This constitutes a progress, enabling researchers to undertake large-scale studies by automating the extensive calculations required by such comparisons of MMDS. Large sample sizes quickly exceed the research resources available, with the result of stagnating research in this area. In addition, statistical inferences can be made regarding patterns of global model configuration. The name “SEXTANT” refers to the purpose of the tool: it should help to analyse characteristics from many articulated MMDS, just as the old orientation device helped seamen to navigate across wide oceans. With SEXTANT, we respond to a call to advance MOC-research beyond small-scale, inductive studies (Hodgkinson, 2002, Huff, 1997, Walsh, 1995).

The paper is organized as follows: Section 2 introduces mental models and especially MMDS. Section 3 explains the process of comparing, analysing, and interpreting MMDSs. In addition, it presents the outlines of selected algorithms in the SEXTANT tool. Section 4 compares the SEXTANT software to other existing software tools. Section 5 discusses the benefits of our software for research and details its limitations. Statements about directions for future work conclude the article.

Section snippets

Mental models of dynamic systems

The term “mental model” was first used by Kenneth Craik (1943) and generally refers to mental representation of causal factors and how they relate to one another. Research about mental representations in the social sciences often has involved human decision-making in dynamic systems. A dynamic system is one that reacts both to interventions of decision-makers and other influences (Edwards, 1962). Mental models as a specific scientific construct originated in psychological research (

Nature of the illustrative study

Earlier in this article, we identified several areas of application within the field of managerial and organizational cognition that would benefit from the application of MMDS elicitation and comparison in large scale studies. Previous investigations into organizational and business-related topics have demonstrated the potential of mental model approaches to further our understanding of organizational learning and the interplay between mental models, resource management, and company success (

Comparison of SEXTANT with alternative software

As was mentioned in the introduction, researchers have developed several software tools to support the elicitation and comparison of mental models. After explaining the principal features of SEXTANT by means of an illustrative example, in this section we briefly compare four well-known tools which deal with mental models to our SEXTANT software.

Discussion

Mental models of dynamic systems (Groesser & Schaffernicht, 2012) are a new concept in the field of managerial and organizational research. Previously, a number of scholars have called on managerial and organizational cognition research to advance beyond the small-scale, inductive studies that have characterizes the field from its beginning (Clarkson and Hodgkinson, 2005, Hodgkinson, 2002, Hodgkinson and Healey, 2008, Huff, 1997, Walsh, 1995).

There is clearly a need to advance research about

References (81)

  • S.M. Brown

    Cognitive mapping and repertory grids for qualitative survey research: Some comparative observations

    Journal of Management Studies

    (1992)
  • I. Clarke et al.

    Management ‘Intuition’: an interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps

    Journal of Management Studies

    (2001)
  • G.P. Clarkson et al.

    Introducing cognizer (TM): A comprehensive computer package for the elicitation and analysis of cause maps

    Organizational Research Methods

    (2005)
  • K. Craik

    The nature of explanation

    (1943)
  • G. Desthieux et al.

    Ulysse: A qualitative tool for eliciting mental models of complex systems

    System Dynamics Review

    (2010)
  • J.K. Doyle

    The cognitive psychology of systems thinking

    System Dynamics Review

    (1997)
  • J.K. Doyle et al.

    Mental models concepts revisited: Some clarifications and a reply to lane

    System Dynamics Review

    (1999)
  • C. Eden

    On the nature of cognitive maps

    Journal of Management Studies

    (1992)
  • C. Eden

    Analyzing cognitive maps to help structure issues or problems

    European Journal of Operational Research

    (2004)
  • C. Eden et al.

    Making strategy: The journey of strategic management

    (1998)
  • C. Eden et al.

    The analysis of cause maps

    Journal of Management Studies

    (1992)
  • C. Eden et al.

    Analyzing and comparing idiographic causal maps

  • C. Eden et al.

    Action research for management research

    British Journal of Management

    (1996)
  • C. Eden

    Using cognitive mapping or strategic options development and analysis (SODA)

  • W. Edwards

    Dynamic decision theory and probabilistic information processing

    Human Factors

    (1962)
  • A.P.J. Ellis

    System breakdown: The role of mental models and transactive memory in the relationship between acute stress and team performance

    Academy of Management Journal

    (2006)
  • M.D. Ensley et al.

    Shared cognition in top management teams: Implications for new venture performance

    Journal of Organizational Behavior

    (2001)
  • C.M. Fiol et al.

    Maps for managers: Where are we? Where do we go from here?

    Journal of Management Studies

    (1992)
  • A. Ford

    Boom and bust in power plant construction: Lessons from the California electricity crisis

    Journal of Industry, Competition and Trade

    (2002)
  • J.W. Forrester

    Principles of systems

    (1968)
  • J.W. Forrester

    Market growth as influenced by capital investment, collected papers of Jay W. Forrester

    (1975)
  • Gary, M., Dosi, G., & Lovallo, D. (2008). Boom and bust behavior: On the persistence of strategic decision biases. In...
  • M.S. Gary et al.

    Mental models, decision rules, and performance heterogeneity

    Strategic Management Journal

    (2011)
  • Y.M. Goh et al.

    Organizational accidents: A systemic model of production versus protection

    Journal of Management Studies

    (2012)
  • S.N. Groesser et al.

    Mental models of dynamic systems: Taking stock and looking ahead

    System Dynamics Review

    (2012)
  • S.N. Groesser

    Mental models of dynamic systems

  • A. Grossler

    Don’t let history repeat itself – Methodological issues concerning the use of simulators in teaching and experimentation

    System Dynamics Review

    (2004)
  • G.P. Hodgkinson

    Comparing managers’ mental models of competition: Why self-report measures of belief similarity won’t do

    Organization Studies

    (2002)
  • G.P. Hodgkinson et al.

    Cognition in organizations

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2008)
  • G.P. Hodgkinson et al.

    Causal cognitive mapping in the organizational strategy field: A comparison of alternative elicitation procedures

    Organizational Research Methods

    (2004)
  • Cited by (15)

    • The importance of eliciting stakeholders’ system boundary perceptions for problem structuring and decision-making

      2022, European Journal of Operational Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      As causal maps allow to represent visually our understanding of mental models, a comparison of causal maps can shed light on important similarities and differences between different groups’ thinking. The comparison of different causal maps improves the understanding of how people comprehend, interpret, and subsequently influence the system in which they operate (Schaffernicht & Grösser, 2014). Grösser and Schaffernicht (2012) highlight that there have been a multitude of approaches proposed for representing and assessing different elements of mental models.

    • An empirical investigation into the learning effects of management flight simulators: A mental models approach

      2017, European Journal of Operational Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      These internal representations have various names: mental models (Doyle & Ford, 1998), mental frames (Isendahl et al., 2009), and mental schemas (Mohammed, Klimoski, & Rentsch, 2000). The concept of mental models has been and remains a central feature of thinking in SD (Doyle & Ford, 1998; Richardson, Andersen, Maxwell, & Stewart, 1994), dynamic decision making (Schaffernicht & Groesser, 2014), learning (Gadgil, Nokes-Malach, & Chi, 2012), and natural resource management (Du Kolkman, Kok, & Van der Veen, 2005; Toit, R., Biggs, & Pollard, 2011). This article takes a SD stance on mental models and how they can be improved.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Tel.: +41 31 848 34 54.

    View full text