Investigation of suitable body parts for wearable vibration feedback in walking navigation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2016.08.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We assessed vibration perception and user preferences on nine body sites.

  • Distal body sites offered the best vibration perceptions.

  • The ears, fingers, wrist and neck are the most preferable.

  • Lower body sites (the feet) were less preferable in walking situations.

  • Proposed suitable vibration positions and design guidelines for walking navigation.

Abstract

Many studies have demonstrated the benefits of wearable vibration devices for walking navigation (Tsukada and Yasumura, 2004). Despite the potential benefits, suitable body parts for wearable vibration devices have not been defined or evaluated until now. We conducted three experiments to identify suitable body parts in terms of perceivability, wearability and user body location preferences for vibration devices. We tested vibration feedback on nine body parts (the ear, neck, chest, waist, wrist, hand, finger, ankle and foot). Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 were conducted in the lab and in real-world walking settings in order to find suitable body parts. Our results indicate that the finger, wrist, ear, neck and feet had the highest perceivability and user preferences. Experiment 3 was conducted to understand the practical usability of those vibration positions in walking navigation. Our study results suggested that the feet are not suitable locations for vibration feedback in walking navigation. Based on the study results, we present design implications and guidelines for wearable vibration devices.

Introduction

Recently, walking navigation systems have become more efficient and user reliance on such systems is on the rise. Researchers (Brewster and Brown, 2004, Brewster et al., 2007 have shown that vibration feedback is especially beneficial in mobile situations where other human senses are distracted by complex environmental factors. Nowadays, many wearable vibration devices are available on the market (Fig. 1) and despite the recognized potential benefit of wearable vibration devices for navigation, a detailed understanding of the suitable bodily positions for vibration devices is lacking. Furthermore, important design factors such as context of use, user comfort, wearability and mobility for navigation have not been adequately explored. Thus, it is important to find the most suitable vibration positions to convey directional information when walking; these positions should be practical and comfortable to wear in real mobile contexts.

In Experiment 1, we tested vibration perception in eight vibration positions: the ear, neck, chest, waist, wrist, hand, ankle and foot (Fig. 2a). However, according to suggestions offered by the participants, the finger was added in Experiment 2. Furthermore, vibration positions on the ear, wrist, hand, finger, ankle and foot were mirrored. Thus the vibration positions in Experiment 2 were increased from 8 to 15 positions (Fig. 2b).

In addition to vibration perceptions, knowledge about interference from movement on each body part was also required to determine the most suitable vibration positions. Previous studies (van Erp, 2005, Jones et al., 2009, Karuei et al., 2011) have demonstrated diminished vibration perception on some body parts. However, these studies were conducted in lab settings. We considered that interference from motion in the lab setting would likely be significantly different from that in real walking settings because real mobile environments impose a load on the visual and auditory senses which may distract the user from paying attention to less sensitive body parts. Thus, to understand suitable vibration positions, we also conducted experiments in a real mobile setting (Experiment 2). Results from Experiments 1 and 2 indicated that the ear, wrist, finger, neck and foot were the most sensitive to vibrations. In Experiment 3, to assess the practical usability of the vibration positions for walking navigation, the participants performed map navigation using the vibration positions suggested from Experiments 1 and 2. Results from Experiment 3 indicated that the feet were less effective for vibration feedback in walking navigation situations.

We addressed three specific research questions.

  • Q1. Among the body parts of interest, which positions offer the best vibration perception in static and walking conditions respectively? (Experiment 1)

  • Q2. Which vibration positions are the most preferable for users to use in real mobile settings? (Experiment 2)

  • Q3. Which vibration positions are practical and usable for walking navigation? (Experiment 3)

The study results were compiled and presented as design guidelines for wearable vibration feedback in walking navigation. The contributions of our study are:

  • Evaluation of vibration perception on different body parts when users are walking in real mobile environments

  • Identification of the most suitable body parts for walking navigation

  • Design guidelines for wearable vibration devices for walking navigation

Section snippets

Related work

For our study, which is focused on vibration feedback for walking navigation, we highlight the most relevant literature in human haptic perception, wearable vibration and walking navigation.

Experiment 1: vibration perceivability in static and walking conditions1

Experiment 1 compares participants' vibration perception on different body parts in varied conditions. The experiment was conducted to address Q1: Among the body parts of interest, which positions offer the best vibration perception in static and walking conditions respectively?

Experiment 2: vibration perceivability in real mobile environments

Experiment 1 investigated vibration perception and the effect of motion on different body parts. We aimed to investigate differences between vibration perception and user preferences in the lab setting and in the real mobile settings in order to determine suitable vibration positions for real-world settings. Thus, Experiment 2 was conducted to address Q2: Which vibration positions are the most preferable for use in real mobile environments?

Experiment 3: practical usability for walking navigation

Experiment 3 sought to assess the potential of each vibrator position (i.e. the wrists, fingers, feet, ears and the neck) as suggested by Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 for application in real navigation situations. Using these vibration positions, the participants performed map navigation (see Fig. 8). Thus, Experiment 3 aims to address Q3: Which vibration positions are practically usable for walking navigation?

Design implications and guidelines

From the study results and close observations throughout the experiments, we propose the following design guidelines.

Conclusion and future work

This study presents appropriate body parts for vibration feedback in order to facilitate the design of effective vibration feedback for walking navigation. The study investigated static and fast walking in the lab setting (Experiment 1), walking in real mobile settings (Experiment 2), and walking navigation (Experiment 3). The main findings are:

  • The distal body parts (i.e., ears, wrist, fingers, feet, neck) deliver better vibration perceivability. These body parts can be used interchangeably for

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Handityo Aulia Putra, Machida Taiga, John Cahill and other members of The Center for Human-Engaged Computing at Kochi University of Technology, for their wide-ranging support. We are also grateful to our participants for their effort and anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful suggestions.

References (38)

  • R.W. Angel et al.

    Velocity-dependent suppression of cutaneous sensitivity during movement

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1982)
  • C. Ho et al.

    Using spatial vibrotactile cues to direct visual attention in driving scenes

    Transport. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav.

    (2005)
  • K.O. Johnson

    The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2001)
  • Alvina, J., Zhao, S., Perrault, S.T., Azh, M., Roumen, T., Fjeld, M., 2015. Omnivib: towards cross-body spatiotemporal...
  • M. Azadi et al.

    Evaluating vibrotactile dimensions for the design of tactons

    IEEE Trans. Haptics

    (2014)
  • Bosman, S., Groenendaal, B., Findlater, J.-W., Visser, T., de Graaf, M., Markopoulos, P., 2003. Gentleguide: an...
  • Brewster, S., Brown, L.M., 2004. Tactons: structured tactile messages for non-visual information display. In:...
  • Brewster, S., Chohan, F., Brown, L., 2007. Tactile feedback for mobile interactions. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI...
  • C. Chapman et al.

    Sensory perception during movement in man

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1987)
  • Ertan, S., Lee, C., Willets, A., Tan, H., Pentland, A., 1998. A wearable haptic navigation guidance system. In: Second...
  • Gemperle, F., Ota, N., Siewiorek, D., 2001. Design of a wearable tactile display. In: Fifth International Symposium on...
  • Heuten, W., Henze, N., Boll, S., Pielot, M., 2008. Tactile wayfinder: a non-visual support system for wayfinding. In:...
  • Holland, S., Morse, D.R., Gedenryd, H., 2001. Audio GPS: spatial audio in a minimal attention interface. Environment 1,...
  • W. Jiang et al.

    Modulation of the cutaneous responsiveness of neurones in the primary somatosensory cortex during conditioned arm movements in the monkey

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1991)
  • K.O. Johnson et al.

    Evaluation of the relative roles of slowly and rapidly adapting afferent fibers in roughness perception

    Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol.

    (1994)
  • L.A. Jones et al.

    Vibrotactile pattern recognition on the arm and back

    Perception

    (2009)
  • Karuei, I., MacLean, K.E., Foley-Fisher, Z., MacKenzie, R., Koch, S., El-Zohairy, M., 2011. Detecting vibrations across...
  • R.L. Knoblauch et al.

    Field studies of pedestrian walking speed and start-up time

    Transport. Res. Rec. J. Transport. Res. Board

    (1996)
  • S.J. Lederman

    Skin and touch

    Encycl. Human Biol. 7

    (1991)
  • Cited by (28)

    • Vibration warning design for reaction time reduction under the environment of intelligent connected vehicles

      2021, Applied Ergonomics
      Citation Excerpt :

      To investigate the effectiveness of vibration at different body parts and S-R compatibility, we chose three vibration positions: wrist, shin, and upper jaw. These are the three regions that previous studies have shown to be easier to perceive vibration (Dim and Ren, 2017) and are also compatible with currently available products (wrist/shin smart bands for life or bone conduction earphones). These products are all wearable at these positions and may prevent missing vibrations due to loose contact.

    • Modality capacity and appropriateness in multimodal display of complex non-semantic information stream

      2019, International Journal of Human Computer Studies
      Citation Excerpt :

      The results of this study suggested that in multimodal display systems (1) complex information, non-semantic serial information of varying magnitude, and multiple targets that demanding parallel processing should be presented as visual signal; (2) non-semantic serial information of varying duration can be presented as audio signal; (3) different vibration signal should differ in location rather than magnitude or duration. For instance, results of this study recommended using haptic feedbacks with varying locations (e.g., Dim and Ren, 2017; Kim et al., 2016; König et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017) rather than those with varying amplitude or duration (Bazett-Jones et al., 2008; Chowdhury and Helali, 2008). Politis et al. (2015) compared the effect multimodal warning information in driving scenarios.

    • Evaluating the cycling comfort on urban roads based on cyclists’ perception of vibration

      2018, Journal of Cleaner Production
      Citation Excerpt :

      Generally, the vibration signals can be captured by installing one or multiple accelerometers on different parts of the bicycle, such as the handlebar, saddle or fork (Chou et al., 2015; Gomes and Savionek, 2014; Olieman et al., 2012). Human perception on vibration varies with the vibrated position since the sensitivity to vibration differs between the body parts (Dim and Ren, 2017). Therefore, the international standard (ISO 2631) of Mechanical Vibration and Shock Evaluation of Human Exposure to Whole-body Vibration (International Organization for Standardization., 1997) classified the vibration into seated position, standing position and recumbent position.

    • Vibration Strength Comfort of Smartwatch in Notification Scene

      2023, Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics)
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text