Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 26, Issue 1, 15 May 2005, Pages 114-122
NeuroImage

An electroencephalographic fingerprint of human sleep

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.020Get rights and content

Abstract

Homeostatic and circadian processes are basic mechanisms of human sleep which challenge the common knowledge of large individual variations in sleep need or differences in circadian types. However, since sleep research has mostly focused on group measures, an approach which emphasizes the similarities between subjects, the biological foundations of the individual differences in normal sleep are still poorly understood. In the present work, we assessed individual differences in a range of EEG frequencies including sigma activity during non-REM sleep (8.0–15.5 Hz range) in a group of 10 subjects who had participated in a slow-wave sleep (SWS) deprivation study. We showed that, like a “fingerprint”, a particular topographic distribution of the electroencephalogram (EEG) power along the antero-posterior cortical axis distinguishes each individual during non-REM sleep. This individual EEG-trait is substantially invariant across six consecutive nights characterized by large experimentally induced changes of sleep architecture. One possible hypothesis is that these EEG invariances can be related to individual differences in genetically determined functional brain anatomy, rather than to sleep-dependent mechanisms.

Introduction

Notwithstanding the increasing use of neuroimaging techniques, electroencephalography is still the most universally employed technique in human sleep research. Quantitative analyses of sleep EEG by spectral analysis have led to the development of the 2-process model of sleep regulation (Borbely, 1982). According to this model, the timing of sleep and wakefulness is regulated by the interaction of a homeostatic, sleep–wake-dependent Process S and a circadian, sleep–wake-independent Process C (Borbely, 1982). Well-established evidence on the homeostatic facet of sleep regulation suggests that slow-wave activity (EEG power in the 0.75–4.5 Hz range) depends on the duration of previous sleep and wakefulness, representing a marker of non-REM sleep intensity (Borbely and Achermann, 2000). This feature of sleep has been consistently shown in a broad range of species, including humans, cats, mice, rats, and squirrels (Tobler, 1995). Manipulations of sleep intensity by means, for example, of sleep deprivation, lead to clear homeostatic recovery processes (Borbely and Achermann, 2000) which do not involve the whole cerebral cortex in the same manner. Indeed, these recovery processes are local in nature, as shown in dolphins (Oleksenko et al., 1992), birds (Rattenborg et al., 1999), mice (Huber et al., 2000), rats (Vyazovskiy et al., 2002), and humans (Ferrara et al., 2002, Finelli et al., 2001a). The most striking regional phenomenon of the human sleep EEG is the hyperfrontality of low-frequency EEG activity during baseline and post deprivation non-REM sleep, probably due to a high “recovery need” of the frontal heteromodal association areas of the cortex (Cajochen et al., 1999, Ferrara et al., 2002, Finelli et al., 2001a).

Homeostatic, circadian and regional EEG changes are basic mechanisms of human sleep that seem to challenge the common notion of large individual variations in sleep need or differences in circadian types. These differences are often ignored or considered as experimental noise, to be actively suppressed through the use of statistical methods that emphasize group rather than individual results. Nevertheless, the recent extensive use of blood flow imaging techniques in the neurosciences made it clear that, for example, a large variability in the detected hemodynamic responses across sessions of the same subject and across subjects is actually present (Aguirre et al., 1998, Handwerker et al., 2004, Wei et al., 2004). The importance of taking such individual differences into account has recently been pointed out also in the sleep research field, at least as regards individual variability in the susceptibility to sleep deprivation (Bell-McGinty et al., 2004, Leproult et al., 2003, Van Dongen et al., 2004). However, although inter-individual differences in neurobehavioral deficits from sleep loss constitute a differential vulnerability trait, its neurobiological correlates have yet to be discovered. The elucidation of some biological mechanisms for the behavioral trait of morningness–eveningness (Duffy et al., 2001), for individual differences in the circadian pacemaker program (i.e., long and short sleepers) (Aeschbach et al., 1996, Aeschbach et al., 2001, Aeschbach et al., 2003) and for the variability of sleep duration in the general population (Aeschbach et al., 2003), also encourages a neurophysiological approach to individual differences in sleep characteristics.

Sleep spindles seem to be a natural candidate for this analysis: they are one of the hallmarks of non-REM sleep, one of the few transient EEG events which are unique to sleep, and it has been reported that their incidence shows great inter-individual differences in humans (Werth et al., 1997). As far as their electrophysiological mechanisms are concerned, sleep spindles depend on variations in membrane potentials of thalamocortical neurons that oscillate in the frequency range of spindles at an intermediate level of hyperpolarization (Steriade, 1999). At the macroscopic EEG level, spindle frequency in humans encompasses the 12–14 Hz range (sigma activity1), although many studies bulk these standard bounds of sigma band (Rechtschaffen and Kales, 1968), including frequency bins traditionally considered to be part of the alpha band (for a review, De Gennaro and Ferrara, 2003). Furthermore, large genotype differences between the relative contribution of power in the sigma range have been found in the non-REM sleep of inbred mice (Franken et al., 1998).

Given the large individual differences in spindle frequency activity in human subjects (Werth et al., 1997) and since it has been suggested that such differences are invariant within individuals and could be related to the individual traits of functional anatomy rather than to sleep-dependent mechanisms (Finelli et al., 2001b), in the present work, we decided to specifically analyze both group and individual differences in a range of EEG frequencies including sigma activity during non-REM sleep [8.0–15.5 Hz range (0.25-Hz resolution)] in a group of 10 subjects who had previously participated in a slow-wave sleep (SWS) deprivation study (Ferrara et al., 1999). The six consecutive nights of the experimental paradigm were characterized by profound differences in quantitative EEG measures, as assessed by spectral analysis of the EEG (Ferrara et al., 2002). Whether or not spindle frequency activity is an individual EEG-trait, we hypothesize that its invariance within individuals will be maintained also during nights with a largely different sleep architecture.

Section snippets

Subjects

The current study was carried out on sleep recordings of normal males who had participated in a SWS deprivation study (Ferrara et al., 2002). Ten normal male subjects [mean age = 23.4 years (SEM = 0.87)] were selected as paid volunteers from a university student population. They reported drinking less than three caffeinated beverages per day, usually sleeping 7–8 h per night with sleep onset between 11:00 p.m. and 12:00 midnight, not taking naps during the day, with no excessive daytime

Group analysis

Although analyzed at a higher frequency resolution and also taking the adaptation night into account, the present group analyses of EEG topography strictly parallel those previously published (Ferrara et al., 2002): hence, they will be briefly summarized here. The results point to significant differences in a wide range of EEG frequencies, comparing both the antero-posterior scalp locations and the different recording nights. Regional changes illustrated in Fig. 1 point to a prevalence of EEG

Individual differences in sigma EEG activity

This study showed that each individual is characterized by a peculiar shape of the sleep EEG power spectra in the sigma range and that this shape remains stable across different nights. The striking invariance in the individual sleep EEG topography pattern appears more noteworthy in the light of the different and considerable modifications of sleep characteristics across the six nights. In fact, the first night was characterized by the well-known “first night effect” that usually leads sleep

Conclusions

Average group measures, which emphasize the similarities between subjects, are only one, albeit the most used, of the possible approaches in the study of the neurophysiological correlates of human sleep. However, this approach does not account for the repertoire of individual patterns that can be important for understanding the variety of ways in which human brain organization underlies behavior. Our results support the view of a genetic control of sleep-related oscillations also in humans, and

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by the MIUR grant Finanziamento per le Ricerche di Ateneo 2003.

References (63)

  • D.A. Handwerker et al.

    Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • H.P. Landolt et al.

    Effect of age on the sleep EEG: slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle-aged men

    Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • L. Palagini et al.

    Independence of sleep EEG responses to GABAergic hypnotics: biological implications

    J. Psychiatr. Res.

    (2000)
  • L.D. Silverstein et al.

    The stability of the sigma sleep spindle

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1976)
  • M. Steriade

    Coherent oscillations and short-term plasticity in corticothalamic networks

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • X. Tan et al.

    High internight reliability of computer-measured NREM delta, sigma, and beta: biological implications

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • X. Tan et al.

    Internight reliability and benchmark values for computer analyses of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) and REM EEG in normal young adult and elderly subjects

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2001)
  • I. Tobler

    Is sleep fundamentally different between mammalian species?

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1995)
  • G.C. Van Baal et al.

    Genetic architecture of EEG power spectra in early life

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1996)
  • H.G. Wei et al.

    Attenuated amplitude of circadian and sleep-dependent modulation of electroencephalographic sleep spindle characteristics in elderly human subjects

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1999)
  • X. Wei et al.

    Functional MRI of auditory verbal working memory: long-term reproducibility analysis

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • E. Werth et al.

    Spindle frequency activity in the sleep EEG: individual differences and topographic distribution

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1997)
  • J. Zygierewicz et al.

    High resolution study of sleep spindles

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • D. Aeschbach et al.

    All-night dynamics of the human sleep EEG

    J. Sleep Res.

    (1993)
  • D. Aeschbach et al.

    Dynamics of slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in the human sleep EEG: effect of midazolam and zopiclone

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (1994)
  • D. Aeschbach et al.

    Homeostatic sleep regulation in habitual short sleepers and long sleepers

    Am. J. Physiol.

    (1996)
  • D. Aeschbach et al.

    A longer biological night in long sleepers than in short sleepers

    J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.

    (2003)
  • S. Bell-McGinty et al.

    Identification and differential vulnerability of a neural network in sleep deprivation

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2004)
  • A.A. Borbely

    A two process model of sleep regulation

    Hum. Neurobiol.

    (1982)
  • A.A. Borbely et al.

    Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation

  • D.P. Brunner et al.

    Changes in sleep and sleep electroencephalogram during pregnancy

    Sleep

    (1994)
  • Cited by (196)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text