Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 45, Issue 1, March 2009, Pages 151-159
NeuroImage

Brain activity for peripheral biological motion in the posterior superior temporal gyrus and the fusiform gyrus: Dependence on visual hemifield and view orientation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.063Get rights and content

Abstract

Biological motion, the movement of the human body presented by a small number of point lights, activates among other regions lining the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and gyrus (pSTG) and of the fusiform gyrus. In previous studies with foveal stimuli the activity in the pSTS/pSTG was often confined to the right hemisphere and bilateral in fusiform gyrus. We presented biological motion stimuli in peripheral vision and measured the BOLD responses with functional MRI to test whether the right dominance in pSTS/pSTG also occurred with peripheral stimuli. We found activation exclusively in the right pSTG for both visual hemifields. In the fusiform gyrus activation was found in both hemispheres and for peripheral stimuli strongest for contralateral stimulation. However, in both fusiform gyri leftward-facing stimuli activated different subfields than rightward-facing stimuli, indicating a clustering of the selectivity for the orientation of the human body form. No such clustering was observed in the pSTG. The results indicate for the fusiform gyrus an organization with respect to the view orientation of the stimulus.

Introduction

The human visual system is equipped with mechanisms sensitive to activities performed by other individuals. For example, humans can easily recognize actions, such as walking, from moving point lights attached to the major joints of an otherwise invisible body (Johansson, 1973). The recognition of such point-light walkers is known as biological motion perception. Many brain imaging studies investigated the neuronal networks underlying biological motion perception. Among others, they identified regions in the posterior bank of the human superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) (Beauchamp et al., 2003, Bonda et al., 1996, Grèzes et al., 2001; Grossman and Blake, Grossman and Blake, 2001, Grossman and Blake, 2002, Grossman and Blake, 2004, Grossman et al., 2005, Michels et al., 2005, Pelphrey et al., 2003, Peuskens et al., 2005, Puce et al., 1998, Santi et al., 2003, Saygin et al., 2004, Thompson et al., 2005) and gyrus (pSTG) (Grèzes et al., 1998, Howard et al., 1996, Santi et al., 2003, Servos et al., 2002, Vaina et al., 2001) and the fusiform gyrus (Beauchamp et al., 2003, Bonda et al., 1996, Grossman and Blake, 2002, Grossman and Blake, 2004, Michels et al., 2005, Peelen and Downing, 2005, Pelphrey et al., 2005, Ptito et al., 2003, Santi et al., 2003, Vaina et al., 2001). Most of these studies reported stronger activation in the right pSTS/pSTG than in the left pSTS/pSTG (Beauchamp et al., 2003, Bonda et al., 1996, Grèzes et al., 1998, Grèzes et al., 2001, Grossman and Blake, 2001, Grossman et al., 2000, Grossman et al., 2005, Pelphrey et al., 2003, Peuskens et al., 2005, Puce et al., 1998, Santi et al., 2003, Wheaton et al., 2004). A possible explanation for this asymmetric activation pattern is a functional lateralization. However, previous imaging studies have used only parafoveal stimuli. Therefore, it is unknown how well the right hemisphere dominance holds up for peripheral stimuli.

The perception of biological motion differs somewhat between foveal and peripheral viewing. Detection of biological motion in random dot noise is more difficult in the periphery than in the parafovea (Ikeda et al., 2005), presumably because of differences in visual grouping processes that are required to join the individual light points into a coherent body structure. Indeed, peripheral discrimination of point-light walkers is good if stimuli are not embedded in noise (Thompson et al., 2007). We have recently observed an asymmetry of the recognition ability of biological motion in the visual periphery in which a walker facing away from fixation is better recognized than a walker facing towards fixation (de Lussanet et al., 2008). This behavioral observation could be traced back to asymmetrical BOLD activation by the walker stimulus in areas of the mirror-neuron system (which also responds to biological motion; Santi et al., 2003, Tai et al., 2004).

Here we use the BOLD activations in posterior temporal cortex to investigate the organization of pSTS/pSTG for peripheral biological motion stimulation. The processing of central and peripheral visual stimuli is organized retinotopically in lower and mid-level visual areas (Engel et al., 1997, Huk et al., 2002, Sereno et al., 1995) and the strongest activations occur usually in the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulus. Higher visual areas, such as pSTS/pSTG, are thought to lack such retinotopy. In monkeys, cells in STPa (presumably homologous to human pSTS) possess large receptive fields that extend to the ipsilateral visual field without any retinotopic organization (Bruce et al., 1981). Cells in STPa respond to peripherally presented biological motion (Oram and Perrett, 1994). Electrophysiological studies in STPa furthermore showed that biological motion sensitive cells often show a preference for a particular orientation (i.e. facing direction) of the walker stimulus, or for a combination of orientation and motion direction of the walker (e.g. facing right and walking forward) (Jellema et al., 2004, Oram and Perrett, 1994, Oram and Perrett, 1996). Other neurons in STPa respond to static views of bodies or faces (Perrett et al., 1991, Perrett et al., 1994). Since biological motion perception may be achieved by the analysis of templates (Lange and Lappe, 2006, Lange et al., 2006) or snapshots (Giese, 2004) of human body configuration it is interesting to investigate any functional specialization within pSTS/pSTG for different orientations of the walker. In the monkey, cells recorded during presentation of walking stimuli did not seem to cluster by their function. Cells with different sensitivities (form, motion, and location) were found within a range of < 1 mm (Jellema et al., 2004). However, the STS region contains a functional organization for objects of different visual categories (Logothetis et al., 1999, Pinsk et al., 2005, Tsao et al., 2003). For instance, Pinsk et al. (2005) reported distinct face and body-selective regions in the posterior and anterior STS.

In contrast to STPa, cells in the inferotemporal cortex (ITC) of the monkey, a possible homologue of the human fusiform gyrus, are anatomically clustered by their function for stimuli of the same object category (Tanaka, 1996, Wang et al., 1998). Wang et al. (1998) recorded responses of ITC cells to different facing directions of profile and front views of faces. The critical features for the activation of single cells were first determined in unit recordings with electrodes. In subsequent optical imaging, Wang et al. (1998) looked for the representation of the critical features and showed that the critical features activated different patchy regions, covering the site of the electrode penetration at which the critical feature had been determined. Because signals in optical imaging reflect average neuronal activities in the examined regions, the optical imaging result indicates a regional clustering of cells in the ITC by their feature selectivity. Some functional clustering is also seen in human fusiform gyrus. For instance, pictures of entire human bodies activate a different region of the fusiform gyrus than images of faces (Peelen and Downing, 2005, Peelen et al., 2006). With respect to template- or snapshot-based models of biological motion perception it is interesting to study responses to body actions with different facing directions in the fusiform gyrus, since the fusiform gyrus may provide shape information about body orientation for the analysis of body motion (Lange and Lappe, 2006).

In the present study we ask whether there are functional sub-regions in the fusiform gyrus that are specific for the facing direction of biological motion. Furthermore, we test earlier findings that the right STS is activated more strongly than the left STS with peripheral biological motion. Third, we investigate the differences in the activation in the fusiform gyrus for ipsilateral and contralateral peripheral stimulation.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twelve right-handed, neurological healthy males (mean age 29.4 ± 5 years) participated in the study. Two of them wore non-magnetic goggles to correct for short-sightedness. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf and all subjects gave written informed consent. Apart from the three participating authors (LM, MdL, and RK), they were not informed about the purpose of the study. Three of the participants were unfamiliar with point-light biological

Behavioral results

The eye movement recordings revealed that for the two participants, who had never seen biological motion before, there were very few saccades in the order of less than 1% of fixations. The analysis of the behavioral responses of all subjects in the facing discrimination task showed a perceptual asymmetry that has been previously reported in a different paper: Walkers facing away from the point of fixation were better recognized than walkers facing towards the point of fixation. This perceptual

Discussion

The present investigation yielded two main results. First, peripheral biological motion stimuli from both visual hemifields activated, beside other areas, the right pSTG. Second, in the fusiform gyrus, but not pSTG, BOLD activation for walkers with different facing directions was anatomically separated in each contralateral hemisphere. The first finding is consistent with many previous studies that showed stronger right-hemispheric pSTS/pSTG activations to parafoveal biological motion stimuli (

Conclusion

In conclusion, we have shown that the fusiform gyrus contains representations of biological motion stimuli with different facing directions. In particular, the location of the activation with the same facing directions activated different parts of the contralateral fusiform gyrus. Our findings indicate that the fusiform gyrus is, additionally to the processing of face and whole body stimuli, specifically engaged in the processing of point-light walker.

Acknowledgments

M.L. was supported by the German Science Foundation, the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research BioFuture Prize, and the EC Projects ECoVision and Eurokinesis. R. J. S. and K.L. were supported by Brain Imaging Center West (BMBF).

References (61)

  • PtitoM. et al.

    Separate neural pathways for contour and biological-motion cues in motion-defined animal shapes

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • ShmuelofL. et al.

    Dissociation between ventral and dorsal fMRI activation during object and action recognition

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • TaiY.F. et al.

    The human premotor cortex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions

    Curr. Biol.

    (2004)
  • WangG. et al.

    Fuctional architecture in monkey inferotemporal cortex revealed by in vivo optical imaging

    Neurosci. Res.

    (1998)
  • WheatonK.J. et al.

    Viewing the motion of human body parts activates different regions of premotor, temporal, and parietal cortex

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • BeauchampM.S. et al.

    fMRI responses to video and point-light displays of moving humans and manipulable objects

    J. Cog. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • BeintemaJ.A. et al.

    Perception of biological motion without local image motion

    Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2002)
  • BeintemaJ.A. et al.

    Perception of biological motion from limited lifetime stimuli

    Perc. Psychophys.

    (2006)
  • BenjaminiY. et al.

    Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach of multiple testing

    J. R. Stat. Soc., Ser. B

    (1995)
  • BondaE. et al.

    Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion

    J. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • BruceC. et al.

    Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1981)
  • BuccinoG. et al.

    Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • de LussanetM.H.E. et al.

    Interaction of visual hemifield and body view in biological motion perception

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • DowningP.E. et al.

    A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body

    Science

    (2001)
  • EngelS. et al.

    Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI

    Cereb. Cortex

    (1997)
  • GauthierI. et al.

    The fusiform ‘face area’ is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level

    J. Cog. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • GieseM.A.

    Neural model for biological movement recognition: a neurophysiologically plausible theory

  • GrèzesJ. et al.

    Top-down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a PET investigation

    Cogn. Neuropsychol.

    (1998)
  • GrossmanE.D. et al.

    Learning to see biological motion: brain activity parallels behavior

    J. Cog. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • GrossmanE.D. et al.

    Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion

    J. Cog. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text