Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 56, Issue 3, 1 June 2011, Pages 1608-1621
NeuroImage

Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: A combined EEG–fMRI study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.017Get rights and content

Abstract

Human performance exhibits strong multi-tasking limitations in simple response time tasks. In the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, where two tasks have to be performed in brief succession, central processing of the second task is delayed when the two tasks are performed at short time intervals. Here, we aimed to probe the cortical network underlying this postponement of central processing by simultaneously recording electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data while 12 subjects performed two simple number-comparison tasks. Behavioral data showed a significant slowing of response times to the second target stimulus at short stimulus-onset asynchronies, together with significant correlations between response times to the first and second target stimulus, i.e., the hallmarks of the PRP effect. The analysis of EEG data showed a significant delay of the post-perceptual P3 component evoked by the second target, which was of similar magnitude as the effect on response times. fMRI data revealed an involvement of parietal and prefrontal regions in dual-task processing. The combined analysis of fMRI and EEG data—based on the trial-by-trial variability of the P3—revealed that BOLD signals in two bilateral regions in the inferior parietal lobe and precentral gyrus significantly covaried with P3 related activity. Our results show that combining neuroimaging methods of high spatial and temporal resolutions can help to identify cortical regions underlying the central bottleneck of information processing, and strengthen the conclusion that fronto-parietal cortical regions participate in a distributed “global neuronal workspace” system that underlies the generation of the P3 component and may be one of the key cerebral underpinnings of the PRP bottleneck.

Research highlights

► We used simultaneous EEG–fMRI to investigate the psychological refractory period. ► The post-perceptual P3 component was significantly delayed at short SOAs. ► BOLD signals in inferior parietal lobe and precentral gyrus covaried with P3 activity.

Introduction

Despite the primate brain's massively distributed processing architecture (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991), reminiscent of the multiple-processor design of parallel computers (Nelson and Bower, 1990), human performance exhibits surprisingly strong limitations in multi-tasking. In one of the simplest multi-tasking experiments (Fig. 1), two target stimuli (T1 and T2, e.g., two tones) are presented in brief succession, and subjects' responses (R1, R2) to both targets are recorded. Under these simple dual-task conditions, response times to the second stimulus (RT2) show a significant increase when the stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) between the two tasks is shortened; response times to the first stimulus (RT1), however, remain largely unaffected by SOA. This classic and widely replicated finding has been dubbed the “psychological refractory period” (PRP), in analogy to post-stimulation refractory phenomena observed in nerves (Telford, 1931). The original hypothesis put forward to explain it was that the “central organizing times” of stimulus processing cannot overlap for two stimuli, and thus have to unfold strictly serially, one after the other (Welford, 1952). This notion of non-overlapping “organizing times”, or in other words, a serial processing stage that acts as a bottleneck of information processing, remains a central ingredient of modern theories of the PRP. The central bottleneck model (Pashler, 1994, Pashler, 1998), which emerged from numerous behavioral experiments, involves three stages of processing: a perceptual (P), a central (C), and a motor (M) stage. According to the model, P and M stages can occur in parallel, while the C stages of two tasks cannot overlap and have to be processed serially. Thus, at short SOAs, central processing for T2 is deferred, or passively queued, until central processing for T1 is completed, and RT2 is increased (Fig. 1).

Various behavioral experiments have associated the central processing stage to response selection, i.e., the mapping between sensory information and motor action (De Jong, 1993, Pashler and Johnston, 1989). Recently, it has been proposed that the C stage can be characterized as a decision-making process based on the noisy integration of evidence (Sigman and Dehaene, 2005). Alternative models argue against a structural bottleneck of stimulus processing, which invariably results in passive queuing of the second stimulus, and instead propose a strong influence of executive control in the strategical monitoring of the two tasks (Logan and Gordon, 2001, Meyer and Kieras, 1997a, Meyer and Kieras, 1997b), or the ability to share processing capacity between them (Navon and Miller, 2002, Tombu and Jolicoeur, 2003). According to shared capacity models, response selection can occur in parallel, but with limited processing resources differentially weighted for one task over the other, resulting in a lag between RT1 and RT2 typical for the PRP. Several dual-task studies have reported behavioral congruency effects that poses a major challenge for the bottleneck model, namely the dependence of RT1 on the response that is required for the second stimulus, referred to as backward crosstalk (as opposed to crosstalk from T1 on RT2). Backward crosstalk effects, which are observed when both tasks are similar [(Logan and Delheimer, 2001, Logan and Schulkind, 2000), but see (Miller, 2006)], are difficult to reconcile with the strictly serial bottleneck model, because they provide evidence that central processing for T2 may start before the C stage for T1 is complete.

To further describe and anatomically locate the cognitive processes underlying the PRP, a rich body of evidence, including data from studies using event-related potentials (ERPs), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and recently computational modeling (Zylberberg et al., 2010), has been accumulated. Studies using fMRI have reported various frontal and parietal regions associated with the PRP (Marois and Ivanoff, 2005), but the results do not appear to converge and strongly depend on the statistical approaches used to test for dual-task-specific effects (Szameitat et al., 2011). Isolating PRP related activity by contrasting dual-task against single-task activity, or alternatively, short SOA against long SOA trials, has highlighted different sets of regions in the lateral frontal, medial frontal, premotor and parietal cerebral cortex. Recently, studies using time-resolved fMRI have reported delayed peaks of activation in the left posterior lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the PRP (Dux et al., 2006), and PRP related temporal variations of activity in the bilateral parietal and frontal regions, respectively (Sigman and Dehaene, 2008). However, the sensitivity and interpretation of these studies also suffers from the low temporal resolution of fMRI.

A number of ERP studies investigating the PRP effect have targeted the amplitude and latency of the P3 (or, P300) component, which is characterized by a positive deflection broadly distributed over the scalp, but with a focus over parietal electrodes (Picton, 1992, Sutton et al., 1965). Recently, it has been proposed that the P3, which has been linked to post-perceptual processes such as the context-updating of working memory (Coles et al., 1985, Donchin and Coles, 1988, Verleger et al., 2005), may be related to the access of a target stimulus to a global neuronal workspace associated with conscious report (Del Cul et al., 2007, Sergent et al., 2005). Based on the delay of the P3 evoked by the second target (T2-P3) some ERP studies have proposed an overlap between the cognitive processes mediating the PRP effect and P3-related processes (Dell'Acqua et al., 2005, Sigman and Dehaene, 2008), while the evidence from other studies, showing a large discrepancy between RT2 and T2-P3 latency modulations, suggests independent sources for PRP and P3 effects (Arnell et al., 2004, Luck, 1998). The latencies of earlier sensory ERP components, such as the P1 and N1, have been reported to remain stimulus-locked to both targets and show no postponement related to the PRP (Brisson and Jolicoeur, 2007, Sigman and Dehaene, 2008).

In the present study, our main aim was to probe the cortical network underlying the PRP by using a combination of simultaneously recorded high-temporal resolution EEG and high-spatial resolution fMRI responses. Due to recent advances in the combination of both neuroimaging methods (Herrmann and Debener, 2008, Laufs et al., 2008), fluctuations in EEG can be correlated to the simultaneously recorded fMRI data on a trial-by-trial basis, thus helping to identify the cerebral networks underlying dynamic changes in ERPs. Based on previous research (Dell'Acqua et al., 2005, Sigman and Dehaene, 2008) which showed a close link between the P3 component and the PRP, we hypothesized that the P3 would covary with the fMRI-BOLD signal in the dual-task situation of our PRP paradigm. By correlating the single-trial amplitudes of the P3 component with the single-trial blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) fMRI signals, we were able to isolate a set of two bilateral homotopic regions in the precentral gyrus and inferior parietal lobe. The PRP paradigm used in our study consisted of two identical number-comparison tasks, thus allowing for an additional analysis of behavioral crosstalk effects (Logan and Delheimer, 2001, Logan and Schulkind, 2000).

Section snippets

Participants

Fourteen male right-handed native French speakers participated in this study, which was conducted at the NeuroSpin neuroimaging center in the CEA campus of Saclay, France. Two subjects had to be excluded due to excessive head movements during the scans and strong residual noise in the EEG after preprocessing which rendered the identification of ERP component topographies impossible. All remaining twelve subjects (mean age 24, range 19 to 28 years) had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All

Behavioral results: PRP effect

In all conditions, response times were comparable for trials with T1 on the left or T1 on the right of fixation. Therefore, we collapsed trials across T1 laterality for all subsequent analyses of behavioral data. Overall, response accuracy rates were high and only slightly, but significantly lower in dual-task trials (94.7% ± 1.0) than in single-task trials (96.3% ± 0.7; t11 = 2.79, p = .018; two-sided paired t-test). In dual-task trials, accuracies for the first response (97.7% ± 0.5) were higher than

Discussion

In this study, our aim was to probe the brain mechanisms underlying the PRP effect in dual-task processing by simultaneously recording EEG and fMRI responses. As expected, our behavioral data showed a lengthening of RT2 and significant RT1–RT2 correlations at short SOAs, which are considered as the hallmarks of the PRP (Pashler, 1994, Pashler and Johnston, 1989). Furthermore, we identified ERP alone, fMRI alone, and joint ERP–fMRI correlates of the PRP effect, which significantly constrain the

Acknowledgments

This experiment was supported by INSERM, CEA, and the Human Frontiers Science Program. It constitutes part of a general research program on functional neuroimaging of the human brain which was sponsored by the Atomic Energy Commission (Denis Le Bihan). GH was supported by a Minerva fellowship (Max Planck Society), and would like to thank Jérémie Mattout for his invaluable support during the early EEG/fMRI recordings at SHFJ in Orsay, and Floris de Lange for the illustration of the hands (Fig. 2

References (90)

  • D. Friedman et al.

    The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2001)
  • C.S. Herrmann et al.

    Simultaneous recording of EEG and BOLD responses: a historical perspective

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2008)
  • G.D. Iannetti et al.

    Simultaneous recording of laser-evoked brain potentials and continuous, high-field functional magnetic resonance imaging in humans

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • Y. Jiang

    Resolving dual-task interference: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • S. Karch et al.

    Separating distinct aspects of the voluntary selection between response alternatives: N2- and P3-related BOLD responses

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • H. Laufs et al.

    Recent advantages in recording electrophysiological data simultaneously with magnetic resonance imaging

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • D. Lehmann et al.

    Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1980)
  • S.J. Luck et al.

    Event-related potential studies of attention

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • H. Mandelkow et al.

    Synchronization facilitates removal of MRI artefacts from concurrent EEG recordings and increases usable bandwidth

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • D. Mantini et al.

    Large-scale brain networks account for sustained and transient activity during target detection

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • R. Marois et al.

    Capacity limits of information processing in the brain

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • L. Naccache et al.

    Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli

    Cognition

    (2001)
  • D. Navon et al.

    Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (2002)
  • M.E. Nelson et al.

    Brain maps and parallel computers

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1990)
  • R.K. Niazy et al.

    Removal of fMRI environment artifacts from EEG data using optimal basis sets

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • J. Sackur et al.

    Semantic processing of neglected numbers

    Cortex

    (2008)
  • T. Schubert et al.

    Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2003)
  • M. Sigman et al.

    Parsing a sequence of brain activations at psychological times using fMRI

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • A. Strobel et al.

    Novelty and target processing during an auditory novelty oddball: a simultaneous event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • A.J. Szameitat et al.

    How to test for dual-task-specific effects in brain imaging studies—an evaluation of potential analysis methods

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • K. Vanderperren et al.

    Removal of BCG artifacts from EEG recordings inside the MR scanner: a comparison of methodological and validation-related aspects

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • T. Warbrick et al.

    Single-trial P3 amplitude and latency informed event-related fMRI models yield different BOLD response patterns to a target detection task

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • K.M. Arnell et al.

    Dissociating sources of dual-task interference using human electrophysiology

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (2004)
  • R. Becker et al.

    Visual evoked potentials recovered from fMRI scan periods

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2005)
  • A.J. Bell et al.

    An information maximisation approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution

    Neural Comput.

    (1995)
  • M. Brett et al.

    Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox

  • B. Brisson et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence of central interference in the control of visuospatial attention

    Psychon. Bull. Rev.

    (2007)
  • M.G.H. Coles et al.

    A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1985)
  • R. De Jong

    Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1993)
  • S. Debener et al.

    Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring

    J. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Imaging unconscious semantic priming

    Nature

    (1998)
  • A. Del Cul et al.

    Brain dynamics underlying the nonlinear threshold for access to consciousness

    PLoS Biol.

    (2007)
  • R. Dell'Acqua et al.

    Central processing overlap modulates P3 latency

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • E. Donchin et al.

    Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1988)
  • J.-C. Dreher et al.

    Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulate and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2003)
  • Cited by (43)

    • Adaptive rescheduling of error monitoring in multitasking

      2021, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Taken together, our results suggest that reliable error detection in dual-tasking is maintained by a mechanism that adaptively reschedules higher-level aspects of error processing to after the completion of the whole dual-task. Our findings indicate that the flexible reorganization of componential processes under dual-tasking is not only a viable strategy to prevent interference between decision processes involved in task execution (Hesselmann et al., 2011; Marti et al., 2015; Meyer and Kieras, 1997; Sigman and Dehaene, 2008), but also serves to reduce interference between task execution and performance monitoring. Also included in the “Material and Methods” section of the manuscript: “All data and analysis scripts are publicly available in an online repository hosted by the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/5ub8z/).”

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text