Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 101, 1 November 2014, Pages 159-167
NeuroImage

Laterality of brain activity during motor imagery is modulated by the provision of source level neurofeedback

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.066Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Repeated sessions of motor imagery with neurofeedback lateralizes brain activity.

  • A no feedback control group lateralizes to ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex.

  • The group receiving feedback lateralizes to contralateral sensorimotor cortex.

Abstract

Motor imagery (MI) may be effective as an adjunct to physical practice for motor skill acquisition. For example, MI is emerging as an effective treatment in stroke neurorehabilitation. As in physical practice, the repetitive activation of neural pathways during MI can drive short- and long-term brain changes that underlie functional recovery. However, the lack of feedback about MI performance may be a factor limiting its effectiveness. The provision of feedback about MI-related brain activity may overcome this limitation by providing the opportunity for individuals to monitor their own performance of this endogenous process. We completed a controlled study to isolate neurofeedback as the factor driving changes in MI-related brain activity across repeated sessions. Eighteen healthy participants took part in 3 sessions comprised of both actual and imagined performance of a button press task. During MI, participants in the neurofeedback group received source level feedback based on activity from the left and right sensorimotor cortex obtained using magnetoencephalography. Participants in the control group received no neurofeedback. MI-related brain activity increased in the sensorimotor cortex contralateral to the imagined movement across sessions in the neurofeedback group, but not in controls. Task performance improved across sessions but did not differ between groups. Our results indicate that the provision of neurofeedback during MI allows healthy individuals to modulate regional brain activity. This finding has the potential to improve the effectiveness of MI as a tool in neurorehabilitation.

Introduction

The acquisition of a motor skill is achieved through alterations in brain activity that occurs as a result of practice (Boe et al., 2012, Doyon and Benali, 2005, Halsband and Lange, 2006). While physical practice is the foundation for motor skill acquisition, motor imagery (MI), the mental rehearsal of physical tasks in the absence of overt muscle contraction (Jeannerod and Frak, 1999), has been shown to be an effective adjunct for skill acquisition in numerous disciplines (Arora et al., 2011, Lebon et al., 2010, Schuster et al., 2011). The similarity in spatial activation patterns observed in the brain between real and imagined movement provides the basis for understanding why MI is an effective adjunct to physical practice (Lacourse et al., 2005, Miller et al., 2010, Orr et al., 2008). Specifically, the repetitive activation of neural pathways during MI forms the basis for short- and long-term plasticity that underlies motor learning (Nudo and Milliken, 1996, Nudo et al., 1996). In addition to facilitating skill acquisition in sport and other skilled motor tasks, MI is emerging as a useful adjunct treatment in neurorehabilitation (Barclay-Goddard et al., 2011, Braun et al., 2006). In particular, MI can be coupled with standard therapies in individuals with upper limb (UL) dysfunction post-stroke to better support functional recovery (Nilsen et al., 2010, Page et al., 2011, Riccio et al., 2010). Coupling MI with standard therapies used in stroke rehabilitation can aid recovery in patients with a range of UL impairment (e.g., good, little or no UL function) owing to the low intensity of resources and decreased physical ‘cost’ required to perform MI (Barclay-Goddard et al., 2011, Braun et al., 2008, Page et al., 2007).

An essential component of skill acquisition is the provision of feedback (Newell, 1991, Newell and Ranganathan, 2009, Winstein, 1991). Feedback permits the assessment of actual versus planned performance, including the identification and correction of errors (Salmoni et al., 1984, Schmidt, 1976). An individual performing MI does not receive feedback however, limiting their knowledge of if, and how well, they are imagining the movement. Thus, the effectiveness of MI may be limited by the lack of feedback. This limitation could be overcome by the provision of feedback to the individual via real-time depiction of the brain activity underlying performance. Further, region-specific neurofeedback could also prove helpful in guiding an individual to modulate the activity of particular brain regions. This feature would be particularly salient in rehabilitative applications, where emerging evidence indicates that the laterality of brain activity parallels the degree of achievable functional recovery (Askim et al., 2009, Chieffo et al., 2013, Dong et al., 2006).

Numerous studies have shown that individuals receiving neurofeedback based on sensor-level analysis of magneto- or electro-encephalography (MEG and EEG respectively) data can modulate task-related brain activity over repeated sessions (Bai et al., 2014, Buch et al., 2008, Ono et al., 2013, Soekadar et al., 2011). While effective for some applications, sensor-level analysis lacks the spatial specificity needed for applications requiring neurofeedback from targeted brain regions. This level of spatial specificity however can be achieved using neurofeedback based on source level brain activity. For example, Florin and colleagues recently demonstrated the use of neurofeedback derived from real-time source level analysis of MEG data to successfully modulate activity in selected brain regions (Florin et al., 2013). This work builds on previous source level neurofeedback studies demonstrating modulation of alpha band power fluctuations (Sudre et al., 2011) and increased coherence between two distinct cortical regions (Ora et al., 2013). Similarly, the provision of neurofeedback using real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has enabled the modulation of brain activity in a region-specific manner including the primary motor cortices (Chiew et al., 2012, deCharms et al., 2004) and anterior cingulate (Caria et al., 2007, deCharms et al., 2005).

It is known that repetition of a task is sufficient to drive changes in brain activity. Neurofeedback studies that do not include a control group who perform MI without neurofeedback cannot disentangle neurofeedback-induced changes in brain activity from the aforementioned practice effect. As such, the inclusion of a no feedback control group is necessary to establish the critical role of neurofeedback in driving changes in brain activity. The lack of a control group in source level MEG or EEG studies creates a knowledge gap related to the role of neurofeedback. Filling this knowledge gap would provide key evidence for the role of MI with neurofeedback in facilitating changes in brain activity.

The present study aimed to identify neurofeedback as the factor driving changes in brain activity during MI. We examined the effect of neurofeedback from the left and right sensorimotor cortex, compared to a no feedback control group, on brain activity underlying MI. A secondary objective was to determine if neurofeedback led to greater improvement in the actual performance of the task being imagined. To achieve these objectives, subjects performed actual and imagined movements over three consecutive days. We hypothesized that, over time, brain activity would lateralize to the sensorimotor cortex contralateral to the imagined movement, with this effect observed for the neurofeedback group only. Further, we hypothesized that the actual task performance would improve in both groups as a function of time, with superior performance observed in the neurofeedback group.

Section snippets

Subjects

Eighteen right handed (Oldfield, 1971) subjects (8 male, 24.7 ± 3.8 years) agreed to participate in the study. All subjects were free of neurological disorder and each provided written, informed consent. Prior to the onset of the study, subjects were screened for compatibility with MEG (e.g., magnetic artifacts) according to institutional procedure. Subjects were randomly assigned to either the neurofeedback (FB) or control group based on the order of recruitment using a table generated prior to

Results

Based on offline analyses, 559 trials were discarded due to the presence of muscle activity detected using EMG. This corresponded to 34.5% of the total number of trials performed (1620). Following this, 232 s of data were removed that contained transient artifacts due to MEG sensor malfunction. This corresponded to 3.1% of the total remaining data.

Discussion

This study used a two-group design to identify neurofeedback as the primary factor driving changes in MI-related brain activity over repeated sessions. Specifically, we showed that neurofeedback from bilateral sensorimotor cortices leads to a more contralateral pattern of MI-related brain activity over sessions, as compared to a no feedback control group. Thus, we establish that providing feedback about the brain activity underlying MI of a unilateral motor task improves an individual's ability

Conclusion

We identify the provision of neurofeedback as the factor driving increased contralateral brain activity during MI of a unilateral motor task. This finding establishes the importance of neurofeedback in improving MI performance. These results provide a foundation for pursuing numerous applications for neurofeedback-guided MI. Specifically, neurofeedback may provide critical information about MI performance to afford an opportunity for patients to modulate regional brain activation. Future work

Acknowledgments

SB acknowledges the salary support from the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada in the form of an Early Career Research Award. AG and SK are supported by NSERC training awards, and AT is supported by a SSHRC post-doctoral fellowship. This work was funded by a grant awarded to SB and TB from the Nova Scotia Health Research Foundation (MED-DI 1551).

References (66)

  • S. Hetu et al.

    The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2013)
  • C.H. Hsu et al.

    Effects of visual complexity and sublexical information in the occipitotemporal cortex in the reading of Chinese phonograms: a single-trial analysis with MEG

    Brain Lang.

    (2011)
  • M. Jeannerod et al.

    Mental imaging of motor activity in humans

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1999)
  • M.G. Lacourse et al.

    Brain activation during execution and motor imagery of novel and skilled sequential hand movements

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • T. Ono et al.

    Daily training with realistic visual feedback improves reproducibility of event-related desynchronisation following hand motor imagery

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2013)
  • G. Pfurtscheller et al.

    Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • G. Pfurtscheller et al.

    Beta rebound after different types of motor imagery in man

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2005)
  • A. Schnitzler et al.

    Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery: a neuromagnetic study

    Neuroimage

    (1997)
  • N. Weisz et al.

    Tonotopic organization of the human auditory cortex probed with frequency-modulated tones

    Hear. Res.

    (2004)
  • H. Akaike

    Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle, Budapest

    (1973)
  • S. Arora et al.

    Mental practice enhances surgical technical skills: a randomized controlled study

    Ann. Surg.

    (2011)
  • T. Askim et al.

    Motor network changes associated with successful motor skill relearning after acute ischemic stroke: a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study

    Neurorehabil. Neural Repair

    (2009)
  • O. Bai et al.

    Effect of real-time cortical feedback in motor imagery-based mental practice training

    NeuroRehabilitation

    (2014)
  • R.E. Barclay-Goddard et al.

    Mental practice for treating upper extremity deficits in individuals with hemiparesis after stroke

    Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.

    (2011)
  • T. Bardouille et al.

    Attention modulates beta oscillations during prolonged tactile stimulation

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • D.M. Bates et al.

    lme4: Linear Mixed-effects Models Using Eigen and S4. p. R Package Version 1.0.4

    (2013)
  • T.J.H. Bovend'eerdt et al.

    Practical research-based guidance for motor imagery practice in neurorehabilitation

    Disabil. Rehabil.

    (2012)
  • S. Braun et al.

    Using mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: a framework

    Clin. Rehabil.

    (2008)
  • E. Buch et al.

    Think to move: a neuromagnetic brain–computer interface (BCI) system for chronic stroke

    Stroke

    (2008)
  • D.J. Davidson

    Functional mixed-effect models for electrophysiological responses

    Neurophysiology

    (2009)
  • R.C. deCharms et al.

    Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2005)
  • Y. Dong et al.

    Motor cortex activation during treatment may predict therapeutic gains in paretic hand function after stroke

    Stroke

    (2006)
  • Cited by (50)

    • Evidence for age-related changes in sensorimotor neuromagnetic responses during cued button pressing in a large open-access dataset

      2019, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Regardless of the mechanism underlying the changes, the findings of our study make it clear that caution should be taken when extrapolating results from the significant number of imaging studies involving younger participants to older populations. For example, there is increasing interest in neurofeedback and brain-computer interface applications that utilize changes in the cortical rhythms (Boe et al., 2014; Gruzelier, 2014; Pfurtscheller and Solis-Escalante, 2009). Our findings question the universal applicability of neurofeedback algorithms developed in younger participants for use in older populations.

    • The potential of real-time fMRI neurofeedback for stroke rehabilitation: A systematic review

      2018, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      The results of this systematic review indicate that stroke patients, like healthy individuals, can learn to control brain activity through neurofeedback, and this might ultimately lead to an improvement of stroke symptoms. This postulation is also confirmed by studies aiming at modulating brain activity and connectivity in stroke with fNIRS (Mihara et al., 2012, 2013), MEG (Boe, Gionfriddo, Kraeutner, Tremblay, & Bardouille, 2014; Buch et al., 2012), or EEG (Ramos-Murguialday et al., 2014; Shindo et al., 2011; Young et al., 2014). Notably, evidence exists for a successful use of EEG neurofeedback for cognitive and motor rehabilitation in stroke, but the effects are not consistent across participants (Bearden, Cassisi, & Pineda, 2003; Cannon, Sherlin, & Lyle, 2010; Doppelmayr, Nosko, & Fink, 2007; Reichert et al., 2016; Rozelle & Budzynski, 1995).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text