Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 102, Part 2, 15 November 2014, Pages 332-344
NeuroImage

Hemispheric lateralization of linguistic prosody recognition in comparison to speech and speaker recognition

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.038Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Linguistic prosody recognition involves temporo-frontal areas in both hemispheres.

  • Lateralization of linguistic prosody recognition depends on control task.

  • Right-lateralization in the contrast of prosody vs. speech recognition

  • Left-lateralization in the contrast of prosody vs. speaker recognition

  • Conjoined activity in left parietal and bilateral IFG for both contrasts

Abstract

Hemispheric specialization for linguistic prosody is a controversial issue. While it is commonly assumed that linguistic prosody and emotional prosody are preferentially processed in the right hemisphere, neuropsychological work directly comparing processes of linguistic prosody and emotional prosody suggests a predominant role of the left hemisphere for linguistic prosody processing. Here, we used two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments to clarify the role of left and right hemispheres in the neural processing of linguistic prosody. In the first experiment, we sought to confirm previous findings showing that linguistic prosody processing compared to other speech-related processes predominantly involves the right hemisphere. Unlike previous studies, we controlled for stimulus influences by employing a prosody and speech task using the same speech material. The second experiment was designed to investigate whether a left-hemispheric involvement in linguistic prosody processing is specific to contrasts between linguistic prosody and emotional prosody or whether it also occurs when linguistic prosody is contrasted against other non-linguistic processes (i.e., speaker recognition). Prosody and speaker tasks were performed on the same stimulus material. In both experiments, linguistic prosody processing was associated with activity in temporal, frontal, parietal and cerebellar regions. Activation in temporo-frontal regions showed differential lateralization depending on whether the control task required recognition of speech or speaker: recognition of linguistic prosody predominantly involved right temporo-frontal areas when it was contrasted against speech recognition; when contrasted against speaker recognition, recognition of linguistic prosody predominantly involved left temporo-frontal areas. The results show that linguistic prosody processing involves functions of both hemispheres and suggest that recognition of linguistic prosody is based on an inter-hemispheric mechanism which exploits both a right-hemispheric sensitivity to pitch information and a left-hemispheric dominance in speech processing.

Introduction

When we listen to speech, we do not only infer meaning from what our communication partners say but also from how they say it. Such prosodic cues are an integral part of human communication providing rich information about the speaker's emotional state (emotional prosody) and the speech message (linguistic prosody) (e.g., Wildgruber et al., 2006). Linguistic prosody signals, for example, the boundaries between phrases, relevant words within a sentence, and whether a sentence is a question or a statement (for review see e.g., Friederici, 2011). How the brain processes linguistic prosody has been investigated by a number of neuroimaging and lesion studies (for review see e.g., Friederici and Alter, 2004, Wildgruber et al., 2006, Witteman et al., 2011, Wong, 2002). One central question of this research is the functional lateralization of linguistic prosody processing. Although linguistic prosody serves various functions in speech comprehension, its neural representation does not consistently conform to the left-hemispheric dominance found for other speech processes including lexical and syntactic processes (e.g., Binder et al., 2009, Friederici, 2011, Leff et al., 2008, McGettigan and Scott, 2012, Scott, 2005, Vigneau et al., 2006). It is still a matter of debate whether there is a hemispheric specialization for linguistic prosody processing or whether it involves both hemispheres to the same extent (e.g., Witteman et al., 2011).

Most of what we currently know about the functional lateralization of linguistic prosody comes from neuropsychological and neuroimaging work investigating linguistic prosody as compared to two other processes: first, speech-related processes, such as sentence processing or word recognition in sentences (reviewed in Friederici, 2011), and second, extra-linguistic prosody functions; that is, emotional prosody in particular when segmental (lexical) information is degraded by filtering of the stimulus (reviewed in Wildgruber et al., 2006). While studies investigating linguistic prosody in comparison to other speech processes suggest a predominant role of right-hemispheric areas for linguistic prosody (Meyer et al., 2002, Plante et al., 2002, Strelnikov et al., 2006), neuroimaging studies comparing linguistic prosody and emotional prosody have failed to find a clear right-lateralization in linguistic prosody processing (Wildgruber et al., 2004; see also Gandour et al., 2003, for the influence of listeners' language background). Instead, it has been shown that left-hemispheric areas are more strongly activated in linguistic as compared to emotional prosody processing. A recent meta-analysis of lesion studies found that damage to both right and left hemispheres can result in impaired recognition of linguistic prosody and emotional prosody (Witteman et al., 2011). For emotional prosody, however, patients with lesions to the right hemisphere were more severely affected than patients with lesions to the left hemisphere. For linguistic prosody, no such differential impairment from lateralized brain lesions was found. In contrast, electroencephalography (EEG) studies showed that event-related potentials (ERPs) specific to linguistic prosody processing are lateralized to the right hemisphere (for review see, Friederici and Alter, 2004). A recent EEG study on lexical tone perception showed that bottom-up processes related to auditory processing of pitch contours are right-lateralized, whereas top-down processing of lexical tone showed left-lateralization (Shuai and Gong, 2014). The behavioral tasks used in lesion studies put a particular emphasis on top-down processes, such as attention, memory and decision making, which might have occluded hemispheric lateralization specific to the processing of linguistic prosody.

Here, we used two fMRI experiments (Fig. 1A) to further clarify the functional role of the right and left hemispheres for linguistic prosody processing. In both experiments, we took a novel approach to the topic. The first experiment was designed to test the right-hemispheric dominance for linguistic prosody as compared to other speech-related processes using a task rather than a stimulus contrast. Previous studies have either used contrasts across different stimulus manipulations (e.g., Humphries et al., 2005, Ischebeck et al., 2008, Meyer et al., 2002, Meyer et al., 2004, Plante et al., 2002) or contrasts across tasks which involved different stimuli (e.g., Strelnikov et al., 2006). Here, we controlled for stimulus influences by employing two tasks (i.e., prosody and speech tasks) that were performed on the same stimulus material (Fig. 1B). This means that the two conditions (prosody task and speech task) only differed in terms of task instructions. In the prosody task, participants were asked to recognize whether the present stimulus is the same or different to the previously presented stimulus in terms of intonation (i.e., question vs. statement intonation; one-back prosody task). Since intonation (i.e., falling and rising pitch contours) occurred over multiple speech segments, participants had to focus on suprasegmental linguistic information in the prosody task. In the speech task, participants were asked to recognize whether the present stimulus is the same or different to the previously presented stimulus in terms of lexical content (one-back speech task). Differences in lexical content between adjacent stimuli occurred on the phonemic level (see Methods section for details). Participants, therefore, had to focus on segmental linguistic information in the speech task. Neural substrates of linguistic prosody processing were assessed by contrasting blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses to the prosody vs. speech task. The second experiment was designed to test whether a left-hemispheric involvement in linguistic prosody processing is specific to contrasts between linguistic prosody and emotional prosody or whether it also occurs when recognition of linguistic prosody is contrasted against other non-linguistic processes, such as speaker recognition. Processing of speaker information has been associated with predominantly right-hemispheric temporal regions (e.g., Belin and Zatorre, 2003, Belin et al., 2000, von Kriegstein et al., 2003, von Kriegstein et al., 2010). To date, the neural substrates of linguistic prosody in contrast to speaker recognition are unknown. One possibility is that prosody and speaker recognition are supported by different areas in the right hemisphere. The other possibility is that the brain areas involved in prosody and speaker recognition overlap to a great extent due to similar acoustic properties that need to be analyzed (e.g., Abberton and Fourcin, 1978, Magnuson and Nusbaum, 2007). To test this, we contrasted BOLD responses to a one-back prosody task with a one-back speaker task, in which participants reported whether the present stimulus is the same or different to the previously presented stimulus in terms of speaker identity (Fig. 1C). As in the first experiment, both tasks were performed on the same stimulus material; prosody and speaker tasks only differed in terms of task instructions.

Based on previous literature, we expected that activation in language-relevant areas in temporal and frontal lobes, including superior and middle temporal gyri as well as inferior frontal gyrus, is associated with linguistic prosody processing in both experiments (for review see, Friederici, 2011, Friederici and Alter, 2004, Wildgruber et al., 2006, Witteman et al., 2011). We expected that this network of regions is lateralized predominantly to the right hemisphere for the contrast of prosody vs. speech task in Experiment 1. For the contrast of prosody vs. speaker task in Experiment 2, we had two alternative hypotheses: either prosody and speaker recognition involve different areas in the right hemisphere in which case prosody processing would be right-lateralized or the neural substrates of prosody and speaker recognition overlap to a great extent in the right hemisphere which could result in a predominant left-hemispheric processing of linguistic prosody. We also present the results of the reverse contrasts (i.e., speech task > prosody task in Experiment 1 and speaker task > prosody task in Experiment 2).

Section snippets

Participants

Seventeen adults (10 female; mean age 24.9 years; age range 20–29 years) participated in Experiment 1. A different group of seventeen adults (9 female; mean age 26.1; age range 22–34 years) was tested in Experiment 2. All of the participants were right-handed [as assessed with the Edinburgh questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971)]. None of the participants had any history of neurological or psychiatric disorder. None of them was trained in a tone language, was a professional musician, or had prior

Prosody recognition contrasted against speech recognition predominantly involved right temporo-frontal areas

Recognition of linguistic prosody as revealed by the contrast of prosody task > speech task in Experiment 1 involved a large network of frontal, temporal, parietal, and cerebellar regions (p < 0.05 FWE corrected, Table 1A, Fig. 2A, top row). In the behavioral data, we found a significant main effect of task on proportion correct values (F(1,16) = 30.83; p < 0.001) with higher mean performance in the speech than in the prosody task (92.67% correct vs. 86.42% correct). To account for these behavioral

Discussion

The present study investigated the hemispheric involvement during the processing of linguistic prosody. It was shown that recognition of linguistic prosody differentially involved right- and left-hemispheric regions depending on whether the control task required recognition of speech or speaker (Fig. 2, Table 1). Recognition of linguistic prosody was found to predominantly involve temporo-frontal areas in the right hemisphere when contrasted against speech recognition (Fig. 2A, top row; Fig. 2

Conclusions

In summary, we showed that recognition of linguistic prosody is supported by a network of brain regions in both hemispheres. However, we found that right and left temporo-frontal areas are differentially involved in the recognition of linguistic prosody depending on whether the control task required recognition of speech or speaker. Our results are in agreement with two theories on the lateralization of linguistic prosody which propose a predominant role of right-hemispheric areas for

Funding

This work was supported by a Max Planck Research Group grant to KVK.

References (63)

  • B.D. McCandliss et al.

    The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2003)
  • C. McGettigan et al.

    Cortical asymmetries in speech perception: what's wrong, what's right and what's left?

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2012)
  • M. Meyer et al.

    Brain activity varies with modulation of dynamic pitch variance in sentence melody

    Brain Lang.

    (2004)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    Assessment and analysis of handedness — Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • L.M. Parsons et al.

    Pitch discrimination in cerebellar patients: evidence for a sensory deficit

    Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • M.D. Pell et al.

    Unilateral brain damage, prosodic comprehension deficits, and the acoustic cues to prosody

    Brain Lang.

    (1997)
  • A. Petacchi et al.

    Increased activation of the human cerebellum during pitch discrimination: a positron emission tomography (PET) study

    Hear. Res.

    (2011)
  • E. Plante et al.

    Dissociating sentential prosody from sentence processing: activation interacts with task demands

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • C.J. Price et al.

    The myth of the visual word form area

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • S.K. Scott

    Auditory processing — speech, space and auditory objects

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2005)
  • M.L. Seghier

    Laterality index in functional MRI: methodological issues

    Magn. Reson. Imaging

    (2008)
  • K.N. Strelnikov et al.

    Prosodic clues to syntactic processing — a PET and ERP study

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • Y.X. Tong et al.

    Neural circuitry underlying sentence-level linguistic prosody

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • M. Vigneau et al.

    Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • K. von Kriegstein et al.

    Modulation of neural responses to speech by directing attention to voices or verbal content

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2003)
  • K. von Kriegstein et al.

    Neural representation of auditory size in the human voice and in sounds from other resonant sources

    Curr. Biol.

    (2007)
  • D. Wildgruber et al.

    Cerebral processing of linguistic and emotional prosody: fMRI studies

    Underst. Emot.

    (2006)
  • M. Wilke et al.

    LI-tool: a new toolbox to assess lateralization in functional MR-data

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2007)
  • M. Wilke et al.

    A combined bootstrap/histogram analysis approach for computing a lateralization index from neuroimaging data

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • J. Witteman et al.

    The nature of hemispheric specialization for linguistic and emotional prosodic perception: a meta-analysis of the lesion literature

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • P.C.M. Wong

    Hemispheric specialization of linguistic pitch patterns

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (39)

    • Intonation guides sentence processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus

      2019, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous fMRI studies, however, have not studied this use of prosody. Rather, fMRI research has focused on types of linguistic prosody which are not as crucial for the syntactic structure of a sentence, such as marking a question or statement (Kreitewolf et al., 2014; Sammler et al., 2015) or placing stress (Kristensen et al., 2013; Perrone-Bertolotti et al., 2013). Moreover, studies often used a low-level or non-linguistic baseline condition rather than a comparable linguistic control task (Meyer et al., 2002, 2004; Plante et al., 2002).

    • Analysing patterns of right brain-hemisphere activity prior to speech articulation for identification of system-directed speech

      2019, Speech Communication
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the scope of this study, we mainly focused on prosodic information. Therefore we have focused on electrical signals from the right brain-hemisphere and on basic prosodic features, as it is generally assumed that the right brain-hemisphere preferentially processes prosody (Kreitewolf et al., 2014; Heilman et al., 2004; MacNeilage et al., 2009; Friederici and Alter, 2004; Shapiro and Danly, 1985; Weintraub et al., 1981; Ross and Mesulam, 1979). For instance, Heilman et al. assessed the brain of a subject with a right medial frontal cerebral infarction and observed an impairment in expressing emotions using prosody, and in comprehension and repetition of prosody (Heilman et al., 2004).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text