Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 119, 1 October 2015, Pages 316-324
NeuroImage

Same clock, different time read-out: Spontaneous brain oscillations and their relationship to deficient coding of cognitive content

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.071Get rights and content

Abstract

Neuronal oscillations provide an efficient means of communication, fostering functional neural states supporting action and reaction. High in the action hierarchy, cognitive abilities are severely compromised in neuropsychiatric disease such as schizophrenia. Current thinking highlights a clocking mechanism provided by the phase of an ongoing slow oscillation that offers a temporal frame for coding of perceptual and computational elements. Yet unclear is whether and how a dysregulated clocking mechanism accounts for diminished cognitive performance. Neuromagnetic oscillatory activity was related to cognitive performance assessed by the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in 58 healthy individuals (HC) and 46 schizophrenia patients (SZ). HC showed a correlation between gamma-band oscillations (> 40 Hz) and working memory performance. This relationship was disrupted in several ways in SZ. First, patients evidenced lower gamma power, poorer working memory performance, and no relationship between these measures. Second, the power spectra were dominated by ~ 10 Hz alpha oscillations with no group differences in amplitude. However, analysis of phase-to-amplitude coupling (PAC) revealed exaggerated clocking of gamma activity by alpha phase in SZ, associated with poor working memory performance. Third, despite entrainment by the same 10 Hz clock, gamma amplitude was abnormally distributed across the duty cycle in SZ, a potential consequence of compromised interneuron inhibition. Fourth, SZ showed over-engagement of a fronto-parietal network measured by gamma phase coherence, suggesting a brain state hindering cognitive output.

Such an endogenous temporal organization may be a core dysfunction in SZ: a segregation/integration input imbalance fostering reduced cognitive performance and compromised behavioral output.

Introduction

Timing is of the essence in brain function (Buzsaki, 2006). Accounts in the literature propose a clocking mechanism in which neuronal activity is coordinated by ongoing brain oscillations. The oscillations play an important modulatory role in information integration by providing spatio-temporal “opportunity” windows of reduced or enhanced excitability patterns (Haegens et al., 2011, Weisz et al., 2014). These patterns in turn interact with input to shape the coding, integration, and distribution of information throughout the brain (Buzsaki and Watson, 2012, He et al., 2010, Jensen et al., 2014). Ongoing brain oscillatory activity is no longer considered mere noise but a temporally coordinated structure that is preserved across species (Buzsaki et al., 2013) and substantially determines cognitive ability (Fries, 2005).

Coordination of neural assemblies is reflected in gamma (> 40 Hz) amplitude clocked by the rhythm (phase) of slower oscillations (Buzsaki and Wang, 2012, Canolty and Knight, 2010). This phenomenon is commonly termed cross-frequency phase-to-amplitude coupling (PAC). It is a mechanism creating “gamma packets” assembled in unison by a lower-frequency clock proposed to support efficient cognitive coding and information transmission in the mammalian brain (Buzsaki and Moser, 2013, Canolty et al., 2006, Jensen and Colgin, 2007). Conceivably, deficits in gamma activity may also manifest in dysfunctional clocking by slow oscillatory activity (Allen et al., 2011). Thus, coordination deficits in controlling coherent gamma activity can hamper network communication and result in deficient cognitive performance.

Studies convincingly demonstrate that brain oscillatory activity is critical for working memory performance (Jensen et al., 2002, Palva et al., 2010, Raghavachari et al., 2001) and is dysregulated in neuropsychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia (Haenschel et al., 2009, Kirihara et al., 2012, Uhlhaas et al., 2008). In addition to task related, spontaneous brain activity may contribute to cognitive performance. Brain signals recorded over several minutes to several hours show a characteristic 1/f function in the power spectrum (Buzsaki, 2006, He et al., 2010), i.e., decreasing power as a function of increasing frequency. This 1/f statistic is an indicator for the presence of a particular temporal dependence within the sampled signal, or nested oscillations (He et al., 2010). Slow once determine faster once and faster once regulate even faster once (Buzsaki, 2006). It is this fundamental and intrinsic temporal coordination that is hypothesized to be behind the aberrant coding of cognitive content in schizophrenia (Buzsaki and Watson, 2012, Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014, Uhlhaas and Singer, 2012). It is altered during both rest and various levels of working memory load suggesting a stable characteristic present across cognitive states (Repovs and Barch, 2012).

The present study tested the hypothesis that altered temporal organization is associated with a dysregulated clocking mechanism as a factor in compromised cognitive performance in schizophrenia. A sample of healthy comparison subjects (HC) served as a normative reference for behavioral and neurophysiological data. Analyses focused on ongoing oscillatory activity during an awake, eyes-open resting-state (RS) condition. First, frequency-domain analysis of magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings served to characterize the relationship of different frequencies to cognitive performance as measured by the MATRICS Consortium Cognitive Battery (MCCB) (Nuechterlein et al., 2008). Second, PAC served to determine temporal organization on a local scale and its relationship to cognitive test performance was examined. Finally, the magnitude of large-scale integration into a coherent brain network was evaluated in order to establish a more global description of the network state.

Section snippets

Participants

Procedures broadly followed those of (Popov et al., 2014). In total, 46 inpatients (18 female, 28 male) with an ICD (International Classification of Diseases) diagnosis of paranoid schizophrenia (SZ, code number F20.0) were recruited at the regional Center for Psychiatry. Inclusion criteria were normal intellectual function (IQ > 75 evaluated by MWT-B (Lehrl, 2005)) and no history of any neurological condition or disorder such as epilepsy or head trauma with loss of consciousness. Symptom

MCCB test performance

Group differences in MCCB performance confirmed cognitive deficits in schizophrenia patients (Group F (1,67) = 56.8, p < 0.001; Domain F (6,402) = 19.3, p < 0.001, HF = 0.87; Group X Domain F (6,402) = 3.59, p < 0.04). Post hoc evaluation confirmed group differences across all cognitive domains (speed of processing t67 = 6.9, p < 0.001; attention t67 = 3.9, p < 0.001; working memory t67 = 4.8, p < 0.001; verbal learning t67 = 5.3, p < 0.001; visual learning t67 = 3.6, p < 0.001; reasoning t67 = 3.5, p < 0.001; social cognition t67 = 

Discussion

Brain oscillations are considered a key phenomenon for understanding how the brain supports cognition. The present report sought to identify a specific relationship between spontaneously generated brain activity and cognitive performance in schizophrenia.

In line with previous reports (Grutzner et al., 2013, Sun et al., 2013, Uhlhaas et al., 2006, Uhlhaas et al., 2011), the amplitude of high-frequency gamma oscillations was abnormally low in the present SZ sample. This finding is of particular

Acknowledgments

This work emerged from a project awarded to Brigitte Rockstroh by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Ro805/14-2). The authors thank Almut Carolus and David Schubring for supervising data collection together with the authors; Ursel Lommen, Vanessa Hirt, Andreas Mühlherr, and Mattias Rack for assistance in data collection; Karl Pröpster and Michael Odenwald for diagnosing patients; and Brigitte Rockstroh and Gregory A. Miller for comments on the paper. The authors declare no competing financial

References (75)

  • K. Kirihara et al.

    Hierarchical organization of gamma and theta oscillatory dynamics in schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • T. Korotkova et al.

    NMDA receptor ablation on parvalbumin-positive interneurons impairs hippocampal synchrony, spatial representations, and working memory

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • D.A. Lewis

    Inhibitory neurons in human cortical circuits: substrate for cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2014)
  • D.A. Lewis et al.

    Cortical parvalbumin interneurons and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • J.E. Lisman et al.

    The theta-gamma neural code

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • E. Maris et al.

    Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2007)
  • G. Nolte et al.

    Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2004)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • T. Popov et al.

    Evoked and induced oscillatory activity contributes to abnormal auditory sensory gating in schizophrenia

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • P. Popova et al.

    Changing facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: effects of training on brain dynamics

    Neuroimage Clin.

    (2014)
  • F. Roux et al.

    Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information?

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2014)
  • R. Sala-Llonch et al.

    Brain connectivity during resting state and subsequent working memory task predicts behavioural performance

    Cortex

    (2012)
  • T. Staudigl et al.

    Theta oscillations at encoding mediate the context-dependent nature of human episodic memory

    Curr. Biol.

    (2013)
  • L. Sun et al.

    Evidence for dysregulated high-frequency oscillations during sensory processing in medication-naive, first episode schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • H. Takeuchi et al.

    Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • C. Tallon-Baudry et al.

    Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (1999)
  • P.J. Uhlhaas et al.

    Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • P.J. Uhlhaas et al.

    A new look at gamma? High- (> 60 Hz) gamma-band activity in cortical networks: function, mechanisms and impairment

    Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol.

    (2011)
  • J. Yamamoto et al.

    Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations

    Cell

    (2014)
  • E.A. Allen et al.

    Components of cross-frequency modulation in health and disease

    Front. Syst. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • C. Andreou et al.

    Increased resting-state gamma-band connectivity in first-episode schizophrenia

    (2014)
  • N. Axmacher et al.

    Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2010)
  • M. Bartos et al.

    Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • G. Buzsaki

    Rythms of the Brain

    (2006)
  • G. Buzsaki et al.

    Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • G. Buzsaki et al.

    Mechanisms of gamma oscillations

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • G. Buzsaki et al.

    Brain rhythms and neural syntax: implications for efficient coding of cognitive content and neuropsychiatric disease

    Dialogues Clin. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • Cited by (15)

    • Do NMDA-R antagonists re-create patterns of spontaneous gamma-band activity in schizophrenia? A systematic review and perspective

      2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      While Ketamine administration was robustly associated with increased gamma-band power in human EEG/MEG-recordings in the large majority of studies, a similar pattern was not observed in ScZ-patients. Thus, n = 8 studies in chronic ScZ reported upregulation of gamma-band activity, while n = 5 studies observed a decrease (Kam et al., 2013; Popov and Popova, 2015; Rutter et al., 2009; Soni et al., 2020; Strelets et al., 2006) and n = 5 studies no difference (Hirano et al., 2015; Kissler et al., 2000; Takahashi et al., 2018; Venables et al., 2009; Zeev-Wolf et al., 2018). Currently, studies examining resting-state gamma-band activity in FEP and CHR-P is small (FEP: n = 9 CHR-P: n = 2) but similar to data in established ScZ, the direction of changes in gamma-band power remained inconclusive.

    • Dorsal and ventral cortices are coupled by cross-frequency interactions during working memory

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      PAC has been identified in a variety of species, including rodents (Tort et al., 2009, 2010), nonhuman primates (Whittingstall and Logothetis, 2009; Spaak et al., 2012), and humans (Canolty and Knight, 2010). Moreover, PAC has been demonstrated during cognitive operations such as item-context binding (Tort et al., 2009), spatial navigation and decision making (Tort et al., 2008), working (Axmacher et al., 2010; Leszczynski et al., 2015) and episodic memory (Staudigl and Hanslmayr, 2013; Park et al., 2016), sleep (Staresina et al., 2015), and resting conditions (Florin and Baillet, 2015) and in a variety of clinical conditions such as Parkinson's disease (van Wijk et al., 2016), schizophrenia (Allen et al., 2011; Kirihara et al., 2012; Popov and Popova, 2015), autism (Berman et al., 2015), and affective disorders (Miskovic et al., 2011). Another view of the relationship between slow and fast oscillatory activity comes from recent work investigating the network properties of directed oscillatory coupling [e.g. Granger causality (GC)] between brain regions in humans and non-human primates.

    • Measuring alterations in oscillatory brain networks in schizophrenia with resting-state MEG: State-of-the-art and methodological challenges

      2017, Clinical Neurophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The authors suggested that this finding highlights the importance of the fronto-temporal network in cognitive processes. Decreased gamma-band power has also been noted in patients during a working memory task over fronto-posterior brain areas (Popov and Popova, 2015). Lastly, using mutual information in source-space, SZ patients have been seen to have disrupted connectivity between the right amygdala and the primary and secondary visual cortices compared to healthy controls in a visual categorization task (Ioannides et al., 2004).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text