Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 125, 15 January 2016, Pages 342-349
NeuroImage

Interacting sources of interference during sensorimotor integration processes

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.075Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The interrelation of different sources of interference is examined.

  • We account for interactive effects of different sources of interference.

  • Theta band power was modulated by interactive inference effects.

  • The underlying functional neuroanatomical network is shown.

Abstract

Every day, a multitude of interfering sensory inputs needs to be integrated and adequately processed using response selection processes. Interference effects are typically investigated using classical paradigms like the Flanker and Simon task. The sources of interference for Flanker and Simon effect are known to be different and according to dual process accounts, two distinct functional pathways are involved in resolving these types of interference. It is an open question how far these sources of interference are related to each other and interact. We investigated this question in a system neurophysiological study utilizing a hybrid paradigm combining both Flanker effect-like and Simon effect-like features. We focus on event-related theta oscillations and use beamforming methods to examine functional neuroanatomical networks. The results show that Simon and Flanker interference interacted in a non-additive fashion by modulating theta band activity, probably reflecting the recruitment of cognitive control processes. Beamforming source reconstruction revealed that theta band activity was related to a broad neuronal network comprising prefrontal and cerebellar regions, including the MFG, SFG, IFG, and SMA. These regions were connected to interference processing and conflict resolution, but differed in the amount of specificity for different sources of interference.

Introduction

Our everyday environment constantly confronts us with a multitude of sensory inputs that need to be transformed into appropriate motor responses. The required sensorimotor integration and response selection processes can be complicated when it is difficult to map sensory information to the appropriate response. In cognitive psychology, such stimulus–response compatibility effects have been subject to intense research in the past decades (e.g., Fitts and Seeger, 1953, Hommel, 1997, Ivanoff et al., 2014, Kornblum et al., 1990, Kornblum et al., 1999, Leuthold, 2011). These compatibility effects are frequently investigated using simple reaction time tasks, like the “Flanker task” or the “Simon task” (e.g., Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974, Eriksen, 1995, Keye et al., 2013, Simon, 1990). In all these paradigms, an irrelevant stimulus feature can facilitate the correct response by activating the same response as the relevant stimulus feature (non-conflicting trial), or elicit a response tendency opposite to the correct response (conflicting trial) (Keye et al., 2013). However, the source of interference leading to response conflict is different for these tasks (e.g., Keye et al., 2013, Mansfield et al., 2013). In the Simon task, people make choice responses on the basis of a non-spatial stimulus feature (e.g., a letter or form). Response keys are arranged on a horizontal spatial dimension, and the location of the non-spatial stimulus feature (e.g., letter or form) varies along the same spatial dimension. Conflict effects are higher if task-irrelevant stimulus location and response button position mismatch than if stimulus location and response button position correspond. By contrast, the Flanker task requires people to make choice responses on the basis of a spatial stimulus feature (e.g., an arrow pointing towards to the left or right), but this feature is not shifted along the horizontal dimension. Instead, one or more irrelevant stimuli that are equipped with the same spatial feature are presented alongside the task-relevant target stimulus. Therefore, the source of interference and emerging conflict during sensorimotor processes is due to different sources of information; i.e., stimulus features in the Flanker Task and spatial features in the Simon task.

In line with the notion of separate sources of interference, it has been shown that inference and the processes required to resolve it involve different neurophysiological processes for Simon and Flanker tasks (Mansfield et al., 2013). Other studies have shown that conflict and interference effects are larger for Flanker than for Simon stimuli (Stins et al., 2005, Stoffels and van der Molen, 1988). From a theoretical point of view, interference effects have been suggested to result from a dual-process (e.g., De Jong et al., 1994, Ridderinkhof et al., 1995). It has been assumed that there is direct unconditional perceptual–response activation by features shared between stimuli and responses. The second process is a conditional selection of the relevant feature(s) and the appropriate response due to the stimulus–response binding (e.g., left-pointing arrow = left button press). The differential magnitude of conflict effects in the Flanker and Simon task may be due to the importance of unconditional processes in the Simon task and due to conditional processes in the Flanker task (Mansfield et al., 2013).

Given the evidence suggesting that Flanker and Simon interference effects are distinct from each other and emerge due to distinct cognitive mechanisms, a question standing to reason is in how far these sources of interference (i.e., the conditional and the unconditional route) relate to each other. To examine how Flanker and Simon interferences relate to each other and whether there are additive or non-additive (i.e., interactive) effects of different sources of interference, we used a novel hybrid paradigm combining the features of both Flanker and Simon tasks. In case of additive effects, the Simon interference effect should be similarly modulated by compatible Flanker (cFlanker) and incompatible Flanker (iFlanker) information. However, it is also possible that there are non-additive (interactive) effects: In the case of interactive effects, it is likely that when incompatible Simon (iSimon) information coincides with iFlanker information, the conflict effect (i.e., slowing of response times, decreases in response accuracy) is larger, as compared to a combination where iSimon information coincides with iFlanker information. Hence, it is also conceivable that the conflict effect is smallest in conditions where compatible Simon (cSimon) information coincides with cFlanker information.

To examine the neuronal mechanisms underlying the interrelation of different sources of conflict on a systems level, we focus on event-related neural oscillations in the theta frequency band and use beamforming methods to investigate the underlying neuronal networks. This is done for a number of reasons: Modulations of theta frequency band power have been related to cognitive control processes (Cavanagh et al., 2012, Cavanagh and Frank, 2014, Cohen, 2014, De Blasio and Barry, 2013) that are known to be increasingly required in conflict situations (e.g., Botvinick et al., 2001). Moreover, frontal theta activity can be observed whenever an unexpected uncertainty of correct behavior occurs (i.e., a conflict), most likely signaling the need of cognitive control (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014). Theta oscillations are supposed to establish cognitive control by means of coupling spatially distal sites in the brain (Fries, 2005) to organize relevant neural processes (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004, Uhlhaas et al., 2010, Womelsdorf et al., 2010, Anguera et al., 2013, Cohen et al., 2009, Hanslmayr et al., 2008, Nigbur et al., 2012). Consequently, modulations of theta band activity have frequently been observed during monitoring of conflicting information (Beste et al., 2010, Beste et al., 2011, Beste et al., 2012, Cohen and Donner, 2013, Lavallee et al., 2014, Tang et al., 2013, Wang et al., 2014). We hypothesize that theta band activity is modulated in similar direction as the behavioral data obtained in the combined Flanker–Simon paradigm. Concerning the functional neuroanatomy, we expect that brain regions including the middle and medial frontal gyrus, as well as the superior frontal gyrus to be associated with modulations in theta band activity. These brain regions have frequently been shown to be involved in conflict monitoring processes (Cavanagh et al., 2012, Cavanagh and Frank, 2014, Cohen, 2014, Cohen and Donner, 2013). We hypothesize that the interaction of Flanker-related and Simon-related conflicting information occurs within areas of the middle, medial and the superior frontal gyrus. Given that Flanker interference is due to conditional processes and Simon effects are due to unconditional processes (Mansfield et al., 2013), this would suggest that before-mentioned functional neuroanatomical regions may be the regions where these routes interact.

Section snippets

Sample

In total, 19 participants (10 males, 9 females) were included in the study. The mean age was 24.16 (± 2.17) years and ranged from 21 to 24 years. None of the participants had a history of psychiatric or neurological diseases. All participants were university students and received financial reimbursement. Each participant gave written informed consent prior to study participation and was treated in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the

Behavioral data

The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of “Flanker” and “Simon” on reaction times (RTs). The main effect of “Flanker” indicates that the Flanker-like manipulation was effective (F(1,18) = 40.65; p < .001; η2 = .693): RTs in cFlanker trials were shorter (432 ms ± 13) than RTs in iFlanker trials (481 ms ± 14). As indicated by the main effect of “Simon”, the Simon-like manipulation also was effective (F(1,18) = 109.89; p < .001; η2 = .859): Participants responded faster in cSimon (429 ms ± 12) than in iSimon

Discussion

In the current study, we examined how different sources of interference interact and modulate cognitive control processes. This was done using a hybrid interference paradigm, comprising aspects of both a Flanker- and a Simon task. In this paradigm, a lateralized, Simon task-like target stimulus was preceded by a Flanker task-like, task-irrelevant cue stimulus. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study analyzing the combined effects of Flanker and Simon interference on a system

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) BE4045/10-2 to C.B.

References (68)

  • R. Iannaccone et al.

    Conflict monitoring and error processing: new insights from simultaneous EEG–fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2015)
  • H. Leuthold

    The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: a short review

    Acta Psychol.

    (2011)
  • K.L. Mansfield et al.

    Temporal dynamics of interference in Simon and Eriksen tasks considered within the context of a dual-process model

    Brain Cogn.

    (2013)
  • E. Maris et al.

    Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2007)
  • J.K. Nelson et al.

    Mapping interference resolution across task domains: a shared control process in left inferior frontal gyrus

    Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • R. Nigbur et al.

    Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2011)
  • S. Ocklenburg et al.

    Lateralized neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of response inhibition processes

    NeuroImage

    (2011)
  • F. Perrin et al.

    Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1989)
  • K.R. Ridderinkhof et al.

    Limits on the application of additive factors logic: violations of stage robustness suggest a dual-process architecture to explain flanker effects on target processing

    Acta Psychol.

    (1995)
  • M.F.S. Rushworth et al.

    Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2004)
  • T.R. Schneider et al.

    Enhanced EEG gamma-band activity reflects multisensory semantic matching in visual-to-auditory object priming

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • J.R. Simon

    The effects of an irrelevant directional CUE on human information processing

  • D. Tang et al.

    The neural oscillations of conflict adaptation in the human frontal region

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2013)
  • C.E. Tenke et al.

    Generator localization by current source density (CSD): implications of volume conduction and field closure at intracranial and scalp resolutions

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2012)
  • P.J. Uhlhaas et al.

    Neural synchrony and the development of cortical networks

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2010)
  • K. Wang et al.

    Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing

    NeuroImage

    (2014)
  • J.A. Anguera et al.

    Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults

    Nature

    (2013)
  • D.M. Barch et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2001)
  • M. Bauer et al.

    Tactile spatial attention enhances gamma-band activity in somatosensory cortex and reduces low-frequency activity in parieto-occipital areas

    J. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • C. Beste et al.

    Variations in the TNF-α gene (TNF-α-308G  A) affect attention and action selection mechanisms in a dissociated fashion

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)
  • M.M. Botvinick et al.

    Conflict monitoring and cognitive control

    Psychol. Rev.

    (2001)
  • S.A. Bunge et al.

    Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind

    Brain

    (2001)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks

    Science

    (2004)
  • J.F. Cavanagh et al.

    Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes

    Psychophysiology

    (2012)
  • Cited by (58)

    • Does chronic use of amphetamine-type stimulants impair interference control? – A meta-analysis

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Regarding MDMA studies, total lifetime dosage of MDMA, as an approximation of consumption duration, was inconsistently reported to be associated with executive functions (Roberts et al., 2016b; Zakzanis et al., 2007). Meta-analyses on neuroimaging data in MDMA users do not support an association between serotonin transporter density and lifetime episodes of use (Müller et al., 2019, 2016). The overall inconsistent findings on ATS use duration may be traceable to several major issues: Firstly, ATS-related characteristics usually rely on self-reports, which may not provide a reliable source of information.

    • A role of the norepinephrine system or effort in the interplay of different facets of inhibitory control

      2022, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      These findings can be framed within the “dual-route” account (De Jong et al., 1994) that is, in congruent trials, automated stimulus-response translation processes are in charge of behavior (De Jong et al., 1994; Keye et al., 2013; Kornblum et al., 1990; Mückschel et al., 2016), hindering appropriate response inhibition (Dippel et al., 2016; Young et al., 2018). In incongruent trials, however, a conflict between two mutually exclusive response tendencies emerges; i.e., between the ‘unconditionally automatic’ response tendencies triggered by the stimulus laterality and the controlled conditional selection of stimulus features associated with the correct response button (De Jong et al., 1994; Keye et al., 2013; Kornblum et al., 1990; Mückschel et al., 2016). When action inhibition processes are also required in such a context, it is possible that the pre-activation of “inhibitory control” to cope with conflicting information (i.e., inhibition of interference) increases the likelihood of successfully inhibiting the motor response actions (Chmielewski et al., 2018, 2019; Chmielewski and Beste, 2017).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text