Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 127, 15 February 2016, Pages 74-85
NeuroImage

How learning might strengthen existing visual object representations in human object-selective cortex

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.063Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Object categorization and discrimination change cortical object representations.

  • This change includes an increase in multi-voxel pattern information.

  • This increase is related to a strengthening of the pre-training representations.

Abstract

Visual object perception is an important function in primates which can be fine-tuned by experience, even in adults. Which factors determine the regions and the neurons that are modified by learning is still unclear. Recently, it was proposed that the exact cortical focus and distribution of learning effects might depend upon the pre-learning mapping of relevant functional properties and how this mapping determines the informativeness of neural units for the stimuli and the task to be learned. From this hypothesis we would expect that visual experience would strengthen the pre-learning distributed functional map of the relevant distinctive object properties. Here we present a first test of this prediction in twelve human subjects who were trained in object categorization and differentiation, preceded and followed by a functional magnetic resonance imaging session. Specifically, training increased the distributed multi-voxel pattern information for trained object distinctions in object-selective cortex, resulting in a generalization from pre-training multi-voxel activity patterns to after-training activity patterns. Simulations show that the increased selectivity combined with the inter-session generalization is consistent with a training-induced strengthening of a pre-existing selectivity map. No training-related neural changes were detected in other regions. In sum, training to categorize or individuate objects strengthened pre-existing representations in human object-selective cortex, providing a first indication that the neuroanatomical distribution of learning effects depends upon the pre-learning mapping of visual object properties.

Introduction

Visual object representations in high-level visual cortex are dynamically updated through learning (e.g., Gauthier et al., 1999b, Grill-Spector et al., 2000, Kourtzi et al., 2005, Li et al., 2009, Op de Beeck et al., 2006). Specifically, learning is associated with an increased selectivity for those object properties that are relevant during training (e.g., Folstein et al., 2013, Gillebert et al., 2009, Jiang et al., 2007, van der Linden et al., 2008, Zhang et al., 2010). However, it is still unclear how learning effects are distributed across visual cortex (Bukach et al., 2006, Harel et al., 2010, Op de Beeck and Baker, 2010).

The neural representation of objects and concepts is very complex, with large-scale distributed maps (Haxby et al., 2001, Huth et al., 2012, Konkle and Oliva, 2012, Op de Beeck et al., 2008a), as well as strong focal specificity for faces (e.g., in the FFA) (Grill-Spector, 2003, Kanwisher et al., 1997, Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006), body parts (Downing et al., 2001, Schwarzlose et al., 2005), houses (Epstein et al., 1999) and words (Cohen et al., 2000). Previous studies of learning, focusing either upon laboratory training or upon visual expertise obtained in a more natural setting, have regularly observed learning effects in face-selective regions, in particular in the FFA (Gauthier et al., 1999b, McGugin et al., 2012). In these studies, effects have sometimes also been noted in other functional regions (older study of Gauthier et al.) or in non-face-selective voxels nearby the FFA (McGugin et al., 2012, Wong et al., 2009). There are also studies that did not observe learning effects in face-selective regions, but more in other functional regions such as general object-selective cortex (e.g., the lateral occipital complex or LOC, see Op de Beeck et al., 2006) or even very distributed across visual cortex (Harel et al., 2010).

This heterogeneity illustrates an important point made before (Bukach et al., 2006), namely that the development of visual expertise through experience is a complex phenomenon, characterized by interactions between experience, task demands, and neural architecture. Here, we highlight the potential role of just one of these factors, namely the neural architecture. Recently, it was proposed how the anatomical distribution of learning-induced changes might depend upon the functional neuroanatomy in high-level visual cortex as it exists prior to the learning experience (Op de Beeck, 2012, Op de Beeck and Baker, 2010). Based upon the multidimensional functional properties of brain regions, it is expected that the degree to which each region participates in a particular visual task, and therefore in the learning of this task, varies. More specifically, learning might mostly be associated with changes in the tuning of neural units (neurons or clusters of neurons such as voxels) which are most informative for the learning situation at hand. This informativeness hypothesis predicts a link between the pre-learning functional properties of a neural unit and the effect of learning in this unit (Fig. 1). This prediction has been supported by single-unit recordings in monkeys in the specific context of a simple orientation discrimination task (Raiguel et al., 2006), but the current literature does not deliver direct evidence in the context of object recognition (Op de Beeck and Baker, 2010). Several previous studies, using single-unit recordings (e.g., Sigala and Logothetis, 2002) and human fMRI (e.g., Folstein et al., 2013, Gillebert et al., 2009, Lerner et al., 2008) have already shown that object processing and learning mostly enhances the selectivity for behaviorally relevant stimulus distinctions, but these studies did not relate learning effects to the pre-training selectivity of individual neurons or voxels and they did not directly compare pre- and post-training representations. Here, we present human functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) evidence which is consistent with this informativeness hypothesis in a design which involves training in complex object tasks such as categorization and object differentiation and which allows for an explicit comparison between the pre-learning functional properties in multi-voxel patterns and the effect of learning.

We adapted the design of a previous study (Op de Beeck et al., 2006) in which we investigated how learning affects the general pattern of response for a trained set of objects. Translated to everyday objects, this previous study showed that training to discriminate among cars can change the pattern of response to the category of cars as a whole. However, this previous study could not reveal to what extent effects of training would be related to informativeness, because the study did not include a pre-training measurement (nor post-training) of the selectivity for those stimulus features which were relevant for training, that is, the stimulus properties which differentiate among individual cars. To remediate this problem, the current study targets such selectivity for the fine differences between members of one of the same set of objects. The study includes three sets or ‘types’ of objects, and for each a further distinction between two sub-classes of objects. Subjects were scanned twice, once before and once after subjects were trained to distinguish exemplars within two of the three object types. We included two training regimes, categorization and differentiation, because previous studies have suggested that the training task can influence the type of changes at the neural level (Wong et al., 2009). The third object type was not trained between the two scan sessions, so that the sub-class selectivity for that object type served as a control. Importantly, the stimulus features defining the sub-classes were relevant during training, allowing us to relate training effects to the informativeness of neural units prior to training.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twelve right-handed adults, all female and between the age of 20 and 28, participated in the experiment. All experiments were approved by the committee for medical ethics of the KU Leuven (Commissie Medische Ethiek van Universitaire Ziekenhuizen KU Leuven). An informed consent was collected from all participants, and they received a monetary reward for their time and participation.

Stimuli

The stimuli were created with custom algorithms written in Matlab to produce three unfamiliar types of objects

Results

The experiment included three object types: smoothies, spikies, and cubies (Fig. 2A). Each subject was trained with two object types so that he/she became able to differentiate these stimuli at a level which was subordinate to the distinction between object types. For one trained object type, the training consisted of a categorization task so that all objects of this type had to be divided into two subclasses. For the other trained object type, the training was a non-naming differentiation

Discussion

In the present study, we trained participants to distinguish objects within three novel object types: smoothies, spikies and cubies. During training two types of learning were included, categorization and differentiation, and one object type was not trained. Training was associated with an increased selectivity in object-selective cortex. Furthermore, training was related to an increase in selectivity in a manner which is consistent with the informativeness hypothesis. We did not find any

Conclusion

Summarized, our findings suggest that at least in the tested circumstances, learning to categorize or discriminate previously unknown objects does not involve the creation of truly novel representations and selectivity. Instead, neural changes seem to build on top of existing representations, as such adapting, refining, optimizing, and/or integrating these existing representations. This is consistent with the informativeness hypothesis which has the potential to serve as a general hypothesis

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the European Research Council (Grant ERC-2011-Stg-284101), a Methusalem grant (METH/08/02) from the Flemish Government, and by a Federal Research Action (grant IUAP/PAI P7/11). We thank Chris Baker for feedback on an early version of the manuscript.

References (52)

  • S. Li et al.

    Learning shapes the representation of behavioral choice in the human brain

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • N.N. Oosterhof et al.

    Visuo-motor imagery of specific manual actions: a multi-variate pattern analysis fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2012)
  • H.P. Op de Beeck et al.

    The neural basis of visual object learning

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2010)
  • F.G. Ashby et al.

    Human category learning

    Annu. Rev. Psychol.

    (2005)
  • D.H. Brainard

    The psychophysics toolbox

    Spat. Vis.

    (1997)
  • M. Brants et al.

    Activation of fusiform face area by Greebles is related to face similarity but not expertise

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • C.M. Bukach et al.

    Training experts: individuation without naming is worth it

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (2012)
  • L. Cohen et al.

    The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients

    Brain

    (2000)
  • T. Cukur et al.

    Attention during natural vision warps semantic representation across the human brain

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • K.M. Curby et al.

    A visual short-term memory advantage for objects of expertise

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (2009)
  • P.E. Downing et al.

    A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body

    Science

    (2001)
  • J.R. Folstein et al.

    Category learning increases discriminability of relevant object dimensions in visual cortex

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2013)
  • I. Gauthier et al.

    Unraveling mechanisms for expert object recognition: bridging brain activity and behavior

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (2002)
  • I. Gauthier et al.

    Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition?

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • I. Gauthier et al.

    Activation of the middle fusiform ‘face area’ increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • C.R. Gillebert et al.

    Subordinate categorization enhances the neural selectivity in human object-selective cortex for fine shape differences

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • Cited by (12)

    • Object recognition is enabled by an experience-dependent appraisal of visual features in the brain's value system

      2020, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The principal difference between Gestalt and meaning-based configural principles is that the former are believed to be innate, whilst the latter depend on the previous experience. While the acquisition of new semantic categories may require long training procedures (Gauthier and Tarr, 1997; Seger and Miller, 2010; Brants et al., 2016), learning to recognize a particular visual pattern – as a member of a familiar category – can occur very quickly. In this study, we investigate changes of neural activity underlying a sudden disambiguation of Mooney figures (Mooney and Ferguson, 1951), which can be associated with the salience of the ‘‘Aha!

    • Factors Determining Where Category-Selective Areas Emerge in Visual Cortex

      2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      The effect of stimulus characteristics can be most easily investigated with novel objects: the objects differ strongly in visual features, but the functional domain and the computational requirements are controlled. After relatively short training procedures, studies have found changes in distributed activity patterns that depend upon the visual features of the objects [45,85]. After applying 8 months of training in young monkeys, areas were found with focal selectivity induced by training, and the location of selectivity seemed to bear some similarity across monkeys when trained with the same stimuli [86].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text