Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 167, 15 February 2018, Pages 178-190
NeuroImage

Cortical networks for auditory detection with and without informational masking: Task effects and implications for conscious perception

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.036Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Tone sequences were presented with and without random multi-tone maskers.

  • A fronto-parieto-temporal network was active for actively detected targets.

  • Participants confirmed awareness of non task-relevant, unmasked tones in retrospect.

  • STS and VLPFC were active for all unmasked and all detected tone sequences.

  • These ventral regions may support awareness, whereas dorsal activity was task related.

Abstract

Ambiguous and masked stimuli have been used to study conscious perception by comparing neural activity during different percepts of identical physical stimuli. One limitation of this approach is that it typically requires a reporting task that may engage neural processes beyond those required for conscious perception. Here, we explored potential fMRI correlates of auditory conscious perception with and without overt report. In experiment 1, regular tone patterns were presented as targets under informational masking, and participants reported their percepts on each trial. In experiment 2, regular tone patterns were presented without masking, while the uninformed participants (i) passively fixated, (ii) performed an orthogonal visual task, and (iii) reported trial-wise the presence of the auditory pattern as in experiment 1 (in fixed order). Under informational masking, target-pattern detection was associated with activity in auditory cortex, superior temporal sulcus, and a distributed fronto-parieto-insular network. Unmasked and task-irrelevant tone patterns elicited activity that overlapped with the network observed under informational masking in auditory cortex, the right superior temporal sulcus, and the ventral precentral sulcus in an ROI analysis. We therefore consider these structures candidate regions for a neural substrate of auditory conscious perception. In contrast, activity in the intraparietal sulcus, insula, and dorsal precentral sulcus were only observed for unmasked tone patterns when they were task relevant. These areas therefore appear more closely related to task performance or top-down attention rather than auditory conscious perception, per se.

Introduction

The neural underpinnings of conscious perception are critical for understanding our experience of the world (Block, 2007, Crick and Koch, 1998, Dehaene and Changeux, 2011). An important step towards studying the neural basis of conscious perception has been the use of ambiguous, bistable stimuli (Leopold and Logothetis, 1996), i.e, identical physical stimuli that can be perceived in different ways. Based on the assumption that sensory processes are coupled mainly to physical stimulus properties, neural processes that differ between varying percepts have been deemed strong candidates for neural correlates of conscious perception. This approach revealed a covariation of perception and neural activation patterns in several secondary visual areas (Blake and Logothetis, 2002). In audition, studies have identified a negative-going1 component of the evoked response in auditory cortex that covaries with the conscious perception of a masked tone pattern, but not with the physical presence of individual tones (Gutschalk et al., 2008, Snyder et al., 2015).

Functional imaging studies have observed activity in a distributed, fronto-parietal network during conscious visual detection (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011); this fronto-parietal network has been associated with a global workspace whose activation is thought to be necessary for conscious perception. To correctly dissociate trials with different percepts during functional neuroimaging, participants are usually required to report their percepts on a trial-by-trial basis. By themselves, such reporting tasks are associated with activation in a general task-related network in fMRI (Fox et al., 2005, Hugdahl et al., 2015) and a P3b potential in human EEG recordings (Hillyard et al., 1971). While these task-related responses very likely indicate conscious processing, they are also expected to comprise neural processes that are not necessarily required for the perception, per se (Aru et al., 2012, Pitts et al., 2014). Several workarounds have recently been suggested around this dilemma (Tsuchiya et al., 2015), for example the use of spontaneous eye movements and pupil-dilation responses to reconstruct the timing of perceptual reversals in binocular rivalry (Frassle et al., 2014) in the absence of overt task responses. A number of studies have also suggested involvement of fronto-parietal networks during auditory perception (Eriksson et al., 2007, Giani et al., 2015), but the degree to which these systems are necessarily required for conscious auditory perception remains currently unclear (Dykstra et al., 2017).

Another challenge for understanding the neural basis of conscious perception is its close link with attention (Posner, 1994)2 [but see (Hsieh et al., 2011, Watanabe et al., 2011, Wyart et al., 2011)]. In audition, volitional orienting of attention enhances neural activity in auditory cortex (Hillyard et al., 1973, Rif et al., 1991, Voisin et al., 2006, Mesgarani and Chang, 2012, Seydell-Greenwald et al., 2014), but also in dorsal areas of frontal and parietal cortex (Huang et al., 2012, Krumbholz et al., 2009, Salmi et al., 2007, Shomstein and Yantis, 2004, Uhlig and Gutschalk, 2017). These areas comprise part of the so-called dorsal attention system (Corbetta et al., 1995, Corbetta et al., 2000), and are thought to be important for attentional control. Volitional orienting of attention can facilitate conscious perception of acoustic targets in the presence of informational masking (Leek et al., 1991, Richards and Neff, 2004). Alternatively, attention can also be “captured” by salient stimuli. In this case, the ventral attention system – in which the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) is a key structure (Corbetta et al., 2008) – plays an important role (Alho et al., 2015, Corbetta et al., 2000, Downar et al., 2000, Stevens et al., 2005).

A powerful paradigm to study auditory conscious perception is the detection of a tone pattern in the presence of a random multi-tone masker (Gutschalk et al., 2008), which produces informational masking (Durlach et al., 2003, Kidd et al., 2008). In contrast to energetic masking, which is based on direct spectral competition in the cochlea (Moore, 1995), informational masking can occur in the absence of spectral overlap between target and masker (Fig. 1A), and is highly variable both within and across listeners (Oxenham et al., 2003). When participants report hearing a regular target pattern under informational masking, perceived tones in the pattern evoke a negative-going response (Gutschalk et al., 2008) and enhanced high-gamma activity (Dykstra et al., 2016) in auditory cortex in a latency range from 50 – 250 ms. Enhanced auditory-cortex activity for detected targets under informational masking was also observed in fMRI (Wiegand and Gutschalk, 2012). However, in these studies, a motor response was only given when a target was detected inside the multi-tone masker. While motor-related and sound related activity can be temporally dissociated in MEG and EEG, the contrast of detected minus undetected targets would be expected to reveal motor-related activity in fMRI, limiting the analysis to auditory cortex in our previous fMRI study (Wiegand and Gutschalk, 2012).

Here, we first introduce a modified setup to study auditory detection under informational masking in fMRI with a whole-brain analysis: participants received a visual response cue to ensure similar motor responses for detection, miss, and correct-rejection trials. The paradigm was then used to identify all areas that showed stronger activation for detected versus missed target trials.

A second experiment was performed to explore potential post-perceptual, task-related components of auditory target detection: a similar setup with visual response cue was used, but the auditory stimulus pattern was presented without the multi-tone masker. For this unmasked stimulus, activation was first measured without the auditory task; because the unmasked tone pattern was salient, we expected it to be perceived irrespective of task. Two sets of instructions were used where the unmasked auditory stimuli were not task relevant: once where the presentation was completely passive (fixation), and once where listeners performed an orthogonal, non-demanding visual task. As third instruction, the unmasked stimulus was presented with an auditory detection task, similar to experiment 1. We hypothesized that a potential correlate of conscious auditory perception identified in experiment 1 should also be observed in experiment 2, independent of the task-relevance of the auditory stimulus. Conversely, task-related activity was expected for target-detection trials in the active auditory task, irrespective of the masker's presence.

Section snippets

Participants

In experiment 1, 15 (eight female) participants (aged between 20 and 44 years, mean 24) were included in the final analysis. In experiment 2, 14 (seven female) participants (aged between 20 and 27 years, mean 23) were included in the final analysis. All participants were right handed, reported normal hearing, and no history of otological, neurological, or psychiatric disorders. They provided written informed consent prior to their participation, and the study protocol was approved by the review

Experiment 1

Participants listened to a continuous stream of random tones, which were used as a multi-tone “informational” masker of the target pattern, comprising four identical, isochronous tones (Fig. 1A). Prompted by a color change of a visual response cue, participants indicated by button press whether or not they had detected a target pattern in the interval between the current and prior color change cues (Fig. 1B).

Discussion

The results of experiment 1 confirm previous findings (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011, Eriksson et al., 2007) of an extended fronto-parietal network activated for detected versus missed targets. While it has been suggested that activation in such a distributed network is required for conscious perception (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011), others have argued that at least part of this activity is related to reporting tasks (Aru et al., 2012, Pitts et al., 2014, Tse et al., 2005, Tsuchiya et al., 2015)

Conclusion

The present results identify the right STS and the iPCS as potential constituents of a network supporting conscious auditory perception, in coordination with auditory cortex. While auditory cortex was not the focus of the present study, there is converging evidence that it plays a role for conscious auditory perception (Gutschalk et al., 2008, Dykstra et al., 2017).

Previous studies have implicated that the STS and iPCS are part of a ventral, stimulus driven attention system, but the interaction

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (grant number 01EV0712) to A.G..

References (86)

  • A.S. Giani et al.

    Detecting tones in complex auditory scenes

    Neuroimage

    (2015)
  • A. Gutschalk et al.

    Auditory neglect and related disorders

    Handb. Clin. Neurol.

    (2015)
  • E. Halgren et al.

    Generators of the late cognitive potentials in auditory and visual oddball tasks

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1998)
  • N. Lavie

    The role of perceptual load in visual awareness

    Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • M.J. McGinley et al.

    Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection

    Neuron

    (2015)
  • B.C.J. Moore

    Frequency analysis and masking

  • M.A. Pitts et al.

    Gamma band activity and the P3 reflect post-perceptual processes, not visual awareness

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • D. Poeppel et al.

    Task-induced asymmetry of the auditory evoked M100 neuromagnetic field elicited by speech sounds

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • J. Rif et al.

    Auditory attention affects two different areas in the human supratemporal cortex

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1991)
  • J. Salmi et al.

    Brain networks of bottom-up triggered and top-down controlled shifting of auditory attention

    Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • J.S. Snyder et al.

    Testing domain-general theories of perceptual awareness with auditory brain responses

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2015)
  • N.K. Squires et al.

    Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1975)
  • M.C. Stevens et al.

    Hemispheric differences in hemodynamics elicited by auditory oddball stimuli

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • N. Tsuchiya et al.

    No-report paradigms: extracting the true neural correlates of consciousness

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2015)
  • K. Wiegand et al.

    Correlates of perceptual awareness in human primary auditory cortex revealed by an informational masking experiment

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • G. Aston-Jones et al.

    An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • N. Barascud et al.

    Brain responses in humans reveal ideal observer-like sensitivity to complex acoustic patterns

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2016)
  • H.M. Bharadwaj et al.

    Measuring auditory selective attention using frequency tagging

    Front. Integr. Neurosci.

    (2014)
  • R. Blake et al.

    Visual competition

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • N. Block

    Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (2007)
  • R. Carlyon et al.

    Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (2001)
  • Y.E. Cohen et al.

    Selectivity for the spatial and nonspatial attributes of auditory stimuli in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Spatial neglect and attention networks

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction

    Science

    (1995)
  • F. Crick et al.

    Consciousness and neuroscience

    Cereb. Cortex

    (1998)
  • R. Cusack

    The intraparietal sulcus and perceptual organization

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • R. Desimone et al.

    Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • J. Downar et al.

    A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • J. Duncan et al.

    Restricted attentional capacity within but not between sensory modalities

    Nature

    (1997)
  • N.I. Durlach et al.

    Note on informational masking

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (2003)
  • A.R. Dykstra et al.

    A roadmap for the study of conscious audition and its neural basis

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci.

    (2017)
  • A.R. Dykstra et al.

    Neural correlates of auditory perceptual awareness and release from informational masking recorded directly from human cortex: a case study

    Front. Neurosci.

    (2016)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text