Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 167, 15 February 2018, Pages 121-129
NeuroImage

A common neural network differentially mediates direct and social fear learning

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.039Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We compared Direct (Pavlovian) and social (vicarious) fear learning within participants.

  • Direct and social fear learning activates a common neural network (Amygdala, AI, ACC).

  • The US enters this common network via the Amygdala in direct fear learning, and through the AI in social fear learning.

  • AI more connected to TPJ during social fear learning.

  • Associability gates network connectivity.

Abstract

Across species, fears often spread between individuals through social learning. Yet, little is known about the neural and computational mechanisms underlying social learning. Addressing this question, we compared social and direct (Pavlovian) fear learning showing that they showed indistinguishable behavioral effects, and involved the same cross-modal (self/other) aversive learning network, centered on the amygdala, the anterior insula (AI), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Crucially, the information flow within this network differed between social and direct fear learning. Dynamic causal modeling combined with reinforcement learning modeling revealed that the amygdala and AI provided input to this network during direct and social learning, respectively. Furthermore, the AI gated learning signals based on surprise (associability), which were conveyed to the ACC, in both learning modalities. Our findings provide insights into the mechanisms underlying social fear learning, with implications for understanding common psychological dysfunctions, such as phobias and other anxiety disorders.

Introduction

Humans, and many other animals, can acquire fears through observing conspecifics being subjected to aversive events (Bandura and McClelland, 1977, Debiec et al., 2017, Olsson and Phelps, 2007, Rachman, 1977). This capacity for social learning without direct exposure to aversive consequences is an adaptation that allows the organism to avoid the costs of individual learning, such as predation and poisoning (Laland, 2004, Lindström et al., 2016). Social learning might, however, not always be adaptive: a large proportion of human fears and phobias is acquired through social means, speaking to the generality of this learning mechanism (Askew and Field, 2008, Rachman, 1977).

Despite the cross-species importance of social fear learning, its neural and computational underpinnings are poorly understood, which stands in contrast to our quickly advancing knowledge about fear learning through direct, Pavlovian, conditioning (henceforth direct fear learning). Animal studies of direct fear learning have delineated the amygdala as critical for the acquisition, storage and expression of conditioned fear (LeDoux, 2012). The amygdala is thought to be the site where information about the stimulus predicting danger (the conditioned stimulus, CS) becomes associated with information about the aversive event (the unconditioned stimulus, US). Similarly, research has demonstrated that the amygdala is critical for direct fear learning also in our species (Delgado et al., 2006, LaBar et al., 1998, Phelps and LeDoux, 2005).

Recent studies in rodents (Debiec and Sullivan, 2014, Jeon et al., 2010, Knapska et al., 2006), and fMRI studies in humans (Meffert et al., 2015, Olsson et al., 2007), suggest that the amygdala also plays a central role in acquiring fears through social fear learning. Together with a range of overt behavioral similarities (Olsson and Phelps, 2007), this shared neural circuitry suggests that partially overlapping mechanisms are involved in direct and social fear learning. However, because the available studies have not directly contrasted direct and social forms of fear learning within the same participant, this conclusion has been tentative. This situation mirrors the ongoing debate within the wider field of learning and decision making, where many conclusions about the neural and computational overlap between individual and social experiences are constrained by comparison between studies or reverse inference (Ruff and Fehr, 2014).

In the present study, we addressed the dearth of knowledge about the neural underpinnings of social fear learning by investigating shared and unique features of hemodynamic responses (fMRI) during direct and social fear learning within the same participant. Such a direct comparison within participants is crucial for analyzing the similarities and differences between direct and social fear learning. Moreover, we combined formal learning theory and dynamic causal modeling (DCM) to better understand how the computational mechanisms involved in direct and social fear learning converge and diverge.

The computational mechanisms underlying direct fear conditioning can be well characterized by classical formal learning theory, such as the Rescorla-Wagner (R-W) (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972a) and Pearce-Hall (P-H) models (Pearce and Bouton, 2001) that describe how the CS and US signals are combined algebraically to associate cues with aversive events. The R-W model encompasses the idea of error-driven learning, where the mismatch (i.e., prediction errors) between delivered reinforcements and the organism's predictions of reinforcement leads to updated associations. One basic assumption in the R-W model is that the rate of learning is constant (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972a). In contrast, the P-H model explicitly describe how more surprising outcomes change the rate of learning (termed associability) (Pelley, 2004). Associability increases in proportion to the absolute prediction error on the last interaction with a stimulus, allowing the agent to adapt to changing environments, which by definition, leads to larger prediction errors, and thereby higher associability. These two accounts have recently been combined in a hybrid model, which is closely related to optimal (Bayesian) statistical inference (Roesch et al., 2012). The computations posited by the hybrid model have been linked to neural signals within the amygdala and insular cortex in humans (Boll et al., 2013, Li et al., 2011), as well as the amygdala in rodents (Roesch et al., 2012). These structures implement a surprise-based associability signal, which gates learning from prediction errors when the environment is changing. Whether the amygdala and insular cortex subserve similar roles in social fear learning is presently unknown.

Based on a previously suggested neural model of social fear learning (Olsson and Phelps, 2007), we hypothesized that social fear learning would involve many of the same brain regions as direct fear learning, representing a cross-modal (self/other) core “aversive learning network”. We predicted that the amygdala would be at the center of this aversive learning network, and that one key computational role of the amygdala would be to gate learning based on associability, or attention, signals, in both direct and social fear learning. In addition to the amygdala, this aversive learning network should involve regions linked to the aversive value of pain, both self-experienced and empathic, such as the anterior insula (AI) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), because the US response driving fear learning rests on such representations. Furthermore, because social fear learning by definition must involve distinctly social processes (simply because another individual is the recipient of the US), we predicted that regions commonly involved in social cognition and theory of mind, such as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), the superior temporal sulcus (STS), and the temporo-parietal (TPJ), would be uniquely involved in social fear learning (Olsson and Phelps, 2007). This involvement might either be additive, so that the “aversive learning” network underlying social fear learning would involve additional nodes relative to direct fear learning, or modulatory, so that the same network nodes would receive differential input depending on the modality (self/other).

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-eight healthy adults (15 female. Mean age = 22.8, SD = 3.33), right-handed participants who were free from self-reported life-time psychiatric or neurological disease and medication were recruited. All participants provided written informed consent and were paid 350 SEK (approximately 38 USD) for their participation. Prior to analysis, one participant was removed as this person aborted the experiment during the Direct phase, leaving 27 participants in the analyzed sample. All procedures

Results

To address our predictions about shared and unique neural contributions to direct and social fear learning, we collected whole-brain fMRI data from 27 healthy adults (15 females) undergoing a fear-learning task consisting of one direct phase and one social phase (Fig. 1). In the direct phase, one CS (CS+) was paired with an electric shock (US), applied to the participants wrist, on 50% of the presentations, whereas the other was not (CS-). In the social phase, participants were presented with a

Discussion

In the present study, we sought to understand the computational and neural underpinnings of social fear learning, by directly comparing social fear learning with direct fear learning within the same subjects. We found considerable evidence for a high degree of similarity between social and direct fear learning: the two types of learning were behaviorally indistinguishable, could be explained by the same computational learning model, and involved a cross-modal (self/other) aversive learning

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by an Independent Starting Grant (284366; Emotional Learning in Social Interaction) from the European Research Council (284366), and the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (KAW 2014.0237) to A. Olsson. Björn Lindström was supported by FORTE (COFAS2: 2014-2785 FOIP). Jan Haaker was supported by the German Research Foundation (HA 7470/1-1). Björn Lindström and Jan Haaker wish to thank Rakim Mayers for inspiration.

References (59)

  • K.S. LaBar et al.

    Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study

    Neuron

    (1998)
  • C. Lamm et al.

    Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • J. LeDoux

    Rethinking the emotional brain

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • D.G. McLaren et al.

    A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • T. Nichols et al.

    Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • E.A. Phelps et al.

    Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • S. Rachman

    The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: a critical examination

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1977)
  • K.E. Stephan et al.

    Bayesian model selection for group studies

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • K.E. Stephan et al.

    Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • J. Zaki et al.

    The anatomy of suffering: understanding the relationship between nociceptive and empathic pain

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2016)
  • American Psychiatric Association, A.P.A

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (2013)
  • L.Y. Atlas et al.

    Instructed knowledge shapes feedback-driven aversive learning in striatum and orbitofrontal cortex, but not the amygdala

    Elife

    (2016)
  • D.R. Bach et al.

    Knowing how much you don't know: a neural organization of uncertainty estimates

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • A. Bandura et al.

    Social Learning Theory

    (1977)
  • S. Boll et al.

    Separate amygdala subregions signal surprise and predictiveness during associative fear learning in humans

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • D.W. Bryden et al.

    Attention for learning signals in anterior cingulate cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • J. Debiec et al.

    Intergenerational transmission of emotional trauma through amygdala-dependent mother-to-infant transfer of specific fear

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.

    (2014)
  • B. Deen et al.

    Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2011)
  • M.R. Delgado et al.

    The role of the striatum in aversive learning and aversive prediction errors

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci.

    (2008)
  • Cited by (53)

    • Inferring danger with minimal aversive experience

      2023, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
    • The lifetime impact of stress on fear regulation and cortical function

      2023, Neuropharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Animals will demonstrate a fear response (e.g., freezing) on subsequent exposures to the CS alone, indicating that they have learned that CS predicts the aversive UCS (Fanselow, 1980; Pavlov, 1927). While we will focus on fear learned by directly experiencing a footshock, fear can also be learned indirectly through social inference or the observation of another partner undergoing conditioning in both humans and rodents (e.g., observational or social fear learning; Lindström et al., 2018; Olsson and Phelps, 2007; Yusufishaq and Rosenkranz, 2013). While fear conditioning produces a robust and reliable memory that is evident early in life, several factors can modulate this learning (Gerhard and Meyer, 2021; Maren et al., 1996; Raineki et al., 2010).

    • Large-scale neural network computations and multivariate representations during approach-avoidance conflict decision-making

      2022, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, applying the RW model to a bandit task with concurrent threat and reward allows identifying brain regions that uniquely encode reward vs threat computations (e.g., value expectation, prediction errors), and would formalize approach-avoidance conflict as jointly elevated reward and threat expectations for a given action. While there have been many separate investigations of neurocircuitry encoding reward or threat computations (Daw et al., 2006, 2011; Delgado et al., 2008; Garrison et al., 2013; Homan et al., 2019; Li et al., 2011; Lindström et al., 2018; Niv et al., 2012), there has been a paucity of research directly comparing the computational neurocircuitry of reward and threat (Metereau and Dreher, 2013). Some work investigating neural correlates of prediction error encoding defines valence as the sign of the prediction error signal (Fouragnan et al., 2018).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Equal contribution.

    View full text