Skip to main content
Log in

Speech-Based Disclosure Systems: Effects of Modality, Gender of Prompt, and Gender of User

  • Published:
International Journal of Speech Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Disclosure of personal information is valuable to individuals, governments, and corporations. This experiment explores the role interface design plays in maximizing disclosure. Participants (N = 100) were asked to disclose personal information to a telephone-based speech user interface (SUI) in a 3 (recorded speech vs. synthesized speech vs. text-based interface) by 2 (gender of participant) by 2 (gender of voice) between-participants experiment (with no voice manipulation in the text conditions). Synthetic speech participants exhibited significantly less disclosure and less comfort with the system than text-based or recorded-speech participants. Females were more sensitive to differences between synthetic and recorded speech. There were significant interactions between modality and gender of speech, while there were no gender identification effects. Implications for the design of speech-based information-gathering systems are outlined.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Blau, P. (1986). Exchange and Power in Social Life. New York: Transactions Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beniger, J. (1986). The Control Revolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H. (1996). Using Language. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nass, C. and Gong, L. (2000). Social aspects of speech interfaces from an evolutionary perspective: Experimental research and design implications. Communications of the ACM, 43: 36-43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeves, B. and Nass, C. (1996).The Media Equation: How People Treat Computers, Television, and New Media Like Real People and Places. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E.-J. and Nass, C. (2002). An experimental test of normative group influence and representation effects in computermediated communication: When interacting via computers differs from interacting with computers. Human Communication Research, 28:349-381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamm, C., Walker, M., and Rabiner, L. (1997). The role of speech processing in human-computer intelligent communication. Presented at NSF Workshop on Human-Centered Systems: Information, Interactivity, and Intelligence.

  • Nass, C. and Lee, K.M. (2001). Does computer-synthesized speech manifest personality? Experimental tests of recognition, similarity-attraction, and consistency-attraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7:171-181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nass, C., Foehr, U., Brave, S., and Somoza, M. (2001). The effects of emotion of voice in synthesized and recorded speech. Presented at Proceedings of the AAAI Symposium: Emotional and Intelligent II: The Tangled Knot of Social Cognition, North Falmouth, MA.

  • Tannen, D. (1996). Gender and Discourse. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nass, C., Moon, Y., and Green, N. (1997). Are machines genderneutral? Gender-stereotypic responses to computers with voices. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27:864-876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E.-J., Nass, C., and Brave, S. (2000). Can computer-generated speech have gender? An experimental test of gender stereotypes. Presented at CHI 2000. The Hague, The Netherlands.

  • Moon, Y. (1998). Impression management in computer-based interviews: The effects of input modality, output modality, and distance. Public Opinion Quarterly, 62:610-622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroner, D.G. and Weekes, J.R. (1996). Balanced inventory of desirable responding: Factor structure, reliability, and validity with an offender sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 21:323-333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. and Marlowe, D. (1964). The Approval Motive. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gong, L. (2000). Pairing media-captured human versus computersynthesized humanoid faces and voices for talking heads: A consistency theory for interface agents. Communication. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K.M. (2002). Social-psychological origins of feelings of presence: Creating social presence with machine-generated voices. Presented at International Communication Association, Seoul, Korea.

  • Heeter, C. (1992). Being there: The subjective experience of presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environment, 1:262-271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biocca, F. (1997). The cyborg's dilemma: Progressive embodiment in virtual environments. Journal of Computer-Mediated-Communication, 3. Available at <http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol3/issue2>.

  • Argyle, M. and Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and Mutual Gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazanec, N. and McCall, G.J. (1976). Sex factors and allocation of attention in observing persons. Journal of Psychology, 93:175-180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J.A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85:845-857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J.A. (1984). Nonverbal Sex Differences: Communication Accuracy and Expressive Style. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly,A. (1983). Gender and social influence:Asocial psychological analysis. American Psychologist, 38:971-981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D.W. (1997). Perceiving Talking Faces: From Speech Perception to a Behavioral Principle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassell, J., Sullivan, J., Prevost, S., and Churchill, E. (2000). Embodied Conversational Agents. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panayiotou, G. (1999).Effects of self-focus and evaluation anxiety on task performance. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rickenberg, R. and Reeves, B. (2000). The effects of animated characters on anxiety, task performance, and evaluations of user interfaces. Human Factors in Computing Systems: CHI'00 Conference Proceedings. New York: ACM Press, pp. 49-56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oviatt, S., DeAngeli, A., and Kuhn, K. (1997). Integration and synchronization of input modes during multimodal human-computer interaction. Proceedings of Conference on HumanFactors in Computing Systems (CHI'97). New York, NY: ACM Press, pp. 415-422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oviatt, S. (1999). Ten myths of multimodal interaction. Communications of the ACM 42:74-81.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nass, C., Robles, E., Heenan, C. et al. Speech-Based Disclosure Systems: Effects of Modality, Gender of Prompt, and Gender of User. International Journal of Speech Technology 6, 113–121 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022378312670

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022378312670

Navigation