Skip to main content
Log in

Can scientific impact be judged prospectively? A bibliometric test of Simonton"s model of creative productivity

  • Published:
Scientometrics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Simonton"s (1997) model of creative productivity, based on a blind variation-selection process, predicts scientific impact can only be evaluated retrospectively, after recognition has been achieved. We test this hypothesis using bibliometric data from the Human Factors journal, which gives an award for the best paper published each year. If Simonton"s model is correct, award winning papers would not be cited much more frequently than non-award winning papers, showing that scientific success cannot be judged prospectively. The results generally confirm Simonton"s model. Receipt of the award increases the citation rate of articles, but accounts for only 0.8% to 1.2% of the variance in the citation rate. Consistent with Simonton"s model, the influence of the award on citation rate may reflect a selection process of an elite group of reviewers who are representative of the larger peer group that eventually determines the citation rate of the article. Consistent with Simonton"s model, author productivity accounts for far more variance in the authors" total citation rate (58.9%) and in the citation rate of the authors" most cited article (12.6%) than does award receipt.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bourke, P., Butler, L. (1996), Publication types, citation rates and evaluation, Scientometrics, 37: 473–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1960), Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes, Psychological Review, 67: 380–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., Cole, J. R. (1967), Scientific output and recognition: a study in the operation of the reward system in science, American Sociological Review, 32: 377–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, E., Welljams-Dorof, A. (1992), Of Nobel class: An overview of ISI studies on highly cited authors and Nobel laureates, Theoretical Medicine, 13: 117–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, D. L. (1988), Science as a process: An Evolutionary Account of the Social and Conceptual Development of Science, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, D. L. (1998), Studying the study of science scientifically, Perspectives on Science, 6: 209–231.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Lawani, S. M. (1986), Some bibliometric correlates of quality in scientific research, Scientometrics, 9: 13–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1997), Creative productivity: A predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks, Psychological Review, 104: 66–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonnert, G. (1995), What makes a good scientist? Determinants of peer evaluation among biologists, Social Studies of Science, 25: 35–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinkler, P. (1998), Comparative investigation of frequency and strength of motives toward referencing: The reference threshold model, Scientometrics, 43: 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, H. (1977), Scientific Elite, Free Press: New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lee, J.D., Vicente, K.J., Cassano, A. et al. Can scientific impact be judged prospectively? A bibliometric test of Simonton"s model of creative productivity. Scientometrics 56, 223–232 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021967111530

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021967111530

Keywords

Navigation