Abstract
One of the central questions of spatial reasoning research is whether the underlying processes are inherently visual or spatial. The article reports a dual-task experiment that was conducted to explore the visual and/or spatial nature of human spatial reasoning. The main tasks were inferences based on a spatial version of the interval calculus introduced by Allen (1983). The secondary tasks were presented visually or acoustically, and were either spatial or non-spatial. The results indicate that spatial reasoning is mainly based on the construction and inspection of spatial layouts, whereas no evidence of the involvement of visual representations and processes was found.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Preview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Allen, J. F. (1983). Maintaining knowledge about temporal intervals. Communications of the ACM, 26, 832–843.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A.D. & Lieberman, K. (1980). Spatial working memory. In R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance (Bd. 8). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Byrne, R.M.J. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1989). Spatial reasoning. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 564–575.
Clement, C.,A. & Falmagne, R.J. (1986). Logical reasoning, world knowledge, and mental imagery: Interconnections in cognitive processes. Memory & Cognition, 4, 299–307.
Cohn, A. G. (1997). Qualitative spatial representation and reasoning techniques In KI-97: Advances in Artificial Intelligence (pp. 1–30). Berlin: Springer.
DeSoto, L.B., London, M., & Handel, M.S. (1965). Social reasoning and spatial paralogic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 513–521
Egan, D.E & Grimes-Farrow, D.D. (1982). Differences in mental representations spontaneously adopted for reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 10, 297–307.
Finke, R.A. (1989). Principles in mental imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Freksa, C. (1991). Qualitative spatial reasoning. In D. M. Mark & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Cognitive and linguistic aspects of geographic space (pp. 361–372). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Glasgow, J.I. & Papadias, D. (1992). Computational Imagery, Cognitive Science, 16, 355–394.
Glasgow, J. Narayanan, N.H., & Chandrasekaran, B. (1995) (Eds.). Diagrammatic reasoning. Cambridge: AAAI Press.
Glenberg, A.M., Kruley, P., & Langston, W.E. (1994). Analogical processes in comprehension. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 609–640). New York: Academic Press.
Glenberg, A.M. & Langston, W.E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151.
Gopher, D. & Donchin, E. (1986). Workload-An examination of the concept. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance, Vol. 2, Cognitive processes and performance (pp. 41,1–41,49). New York: Wiley & Sons.
Güsgen, H. W. (1989). Spatial reasoning based on Allen’s temporal logic (Technical Report ICSI TR-89-049). Berkeley, CA: International Computer Science Institute.
Hernández, D. (1994). Qualitative representation of spatial knowledge. Berlin: Springer.
Huttenlocher, J. (1968). Constructing spatial images: A strategy in reasoning. Psychological Review, 75, 550–560.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1972). The three-term series problem. Cognition, 1, 57–82.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1996). Space to think. In P. Bloom et al. (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 437–462). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. (1991). Deduction. Hove (UK): Erlbaum.
Johnson-Laird, P.N., Byrne, R., & Tabossi, P. (1989). Reasoning by model: the case of multiple quantifiers. Psychological Review, 96, 658–673.
Knauff, M. (1999). The cognitive adequacy of Allen’s interval calculus for qualitative spatial representation and reasoning. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 3, 261–290.
Knauff, M. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (2000). Visual and spatial representations in spatial reasoning. In Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 759–765). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Knauff, M., Jola, C., Strube, G., Rauh, R., & Schlieder, C. (2000). Visuo-spatial working memory involvement in spatial reasoning. Under review.
Knauff, M., Rauh, R., & Renz, J. (1997). A cognitive assessment of topological spatial relations: Results from an empirical investigation. In S. C. Hirtle & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Spatial information theory. A theoretical basis for GIS. Proceedings of COSIT’ 97 (pp.193–206). New York: Springer.
Knauff, M., Rauh, R., & Schlieder, C. (1995). Preferred mental models in qualitative spatial reasoning: A cognitive assessment of Allen’s calculus. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 200–205). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Knauff, M., Rauh, R., Schlieder, C., & Strube, G. (1998a). Mental models in spatial reasoning. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial Cognition–An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 267–291). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Bd. 1404. Berlin: Springer.
Knauff, M., Rauh, R., Schlieder, C., & Strube, G. (1998b). Continuity effect and figural bias in spatial relational inference. In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 573–578). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kosslyn, S.M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kosslyn, S.M. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Logie, R.H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mukerjee, A. & Joe, G. (1990). A qualitative model for space. Proceedings AAAI-90, 721–727.
Newstead, S.E., Pollard P., & Griggs, R.A. (1986). Response bias in relational reasoning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 95–98.
Pearson, D.G., Logie, R.H., & Gillhooly, K.J. (1999). Verbal representations and spatial manipulation during mental synthesis. European Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11A, 295–314.
Potts, G.R. & Scholz, K.W. (1975). The internal representation of a three-term series problem. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 439–452.
Richardson, J.T.E. (1987). The role of mental imagery in models of transitive inference. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 189–203.
Shaver, P., Pierson, L., & Lang, S. (1974). Converging evidence for the functional significance of imagery in problem solving. Cognition, 3, 359–375.
Shepard, R. N. & Cooper, L. A. (1982). Mental images and their transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stein, L.A. (1994). Imagination and situated cognition. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Intelligence, 6, 393–407.
Sternberg, R. J. (1980). Representation and process in linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 119–159.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2001 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg
About this paper
Cite this paper
Knauff, M., Jola, C., Strube, G. (2001). Spatial Reasoning: No Need for Visual Information. In: Montello, D.R. (eds) Spatial Information Theory. COSIT 2001. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 2205. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-45424-1_30
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-45424-1_30
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg
Print ISBN: 978-3-540-42613-4
Online ISBN: 978-3-540-45424-3
eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive