Skip to main content

The Route Direction Effect and its Constraints

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Spatial Cognition

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Computer Science ((LNAI,volume 1404))

Abstract

The route direction effect can be characterized as follows: When a person has learned a route and imagines that he or she is at a specific point along this route, then it is easier for him or her to imagine him or her-self at another point on the route which lies in the direction in which the route was learned than the other way around. This means that route knowledge includes information on the route if acquisition, i.e. the direction of acquisition is co-represented in cognitive maps. Within a network theory approach, the route direction effect can be conceived of as a specific asymmetry in the spread of activation. Priming-experiments were carried out to determine a time window for the experimental realization of asymmetrical activation spread. We were actually able to show that the route direction effect disappears when the Stimulus Onset Asynchrony is extended. This was to be expected because a longer SOA causes the target to be pre-activated, regardless of whether it represents an object which was positioned in the direction of acquisition, or not. In further experiments we showed that if route knowledge is gained via a gradient sequence, then normal ecological conditions of perception must have existed: The direction of perception must correspond to the direction of movement. Objects, as well as their surroundings, must be perceived within the normal optical flow. Otherwise the information about the direction of acquisition cannot be adequately incorporated, and the route direction effect does not occur.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

6. References

  • Aginsky, V., Harris, C., Rensink, R., & Beusmans, J. (1996). Two strategies for learning in a driving simulator, Technical Report CBR TR 96-6. Cambridge: Cambridge Basic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, M.A.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Appleyard, D. (1970). Styles and methods of structuring a city. Environment and Behavior, 2, 100–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beller, H. K. (1971). Priming: effects of advance information on matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 176–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A. (1989). Models of psychological time. In R.A. Block (Ed.) Cognitive models of psychological time (pp. 1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, V., & Green, R. R. (1985). Visual perception, physiology, psychology and ecology. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruns, A. (1996). Zum Einfluß des Blickpunkts beim kognitiven Kartieren. Unveröffentl. Diplomarbeit, Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buhl, H. M. (1996). Wissenserwerb und Raumreferenz. Ein sprachpsychologischer Zugang zur mentalen Repräsentation. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. (1989). Memory in the real world. Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (1977). Maps in minds. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1985). Über das Gedächtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie (1. Aufl., Leipzig 1885). Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelkamp, J. (1990). Das menschliche Gedächtnis. Göttingen: Verlag für Psychologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W. (1980). Environmental cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1985). Psychologie der Zeit. München: Ernst Reinhardt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. G., & Gilovich, T. (1989). Effect of memory perspective on retrospective causal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 399–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, N., Tversky, B., & Coon, V. (1992). Switching point of views in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition, 20, 507–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freksa, C., & Habel, C. (Eds.). (1990). Repräsentation und Verarbeitung räumlichen Wissens. Berlin: Springer.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as a perceptual problem. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helm, G. (1991). Symbolische und konnektionistische Modelle der menschlichen Informationsverarbeitung. Eine kritische Gegenüberstellung. Berlin: Springer.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th. (1996). Blickpunkte und Blickpunktsequenzen. Sprache & Kognition, 15, 159–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th., Buhl, H. M., & Schweizer, K. (1995). Zur blickpunktbezogenen Wissensrepräsentation: Der Richtungseffekt. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 203, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th., Grabowski, J., Schweizer, K. & Graf, R. (1996). Die mentale Repräsentation von Konzepten, Wörtern und Figuren (pp. 120–152). In J. Grabowski, G. Harras & Th. Herrmann (Eds.), Bedeutung-Konzepte-Bedeutungskonzepte. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th., Schweizer, K., Buhl, H. M., & Janzen, G. (1995). Ankereffekt und Richtungseffekt-Forschungsbericht (Arbeiten der Forschungsgruppe „Sprache und Kognition“, Bericht Nr. 54). Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, Th., Schweizer, K., Janzen, G., & Katz, S. (1997). Routen-und Überblickswissen-Konzeptuelle Überlegungen-(Arbeiten des Mannheimer Teilprojekts „Determinaten des Richtungseffekts“ im SPP Raumkognition, Bericht Nr.1). Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. G., & Anderson, J. A. (Eds.). (1989). Parallel models of associative memory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, B. K. P., & Schunck, B. G. (1981). Determing optical flow. Artificial Intelligence, 17, 185–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janzen, G. (1996). Wahrnehmungsrichtung und Bewegungsrichtung: Haben Gibsonsche Gradienten einen Einfluß auf den Richtungseffekt? Unveröffentl. Diplomarbeit. Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kato, K. (1982). A study on route learning: the simulation technique by videotape presentation. Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 41, 24–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, S. (1995). Der Ankereffekt und der Richtungseffekt. Unveröffentl. Diplomarbeit, Mannheim, Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, N.-G., Growney, R., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Optical flow not retinal flow is the basis of wayfinding by foot. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1279–1288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluwe, R. H. (1990). Gedächtnis und Wissen. In H. Spada (Ed.), Lehrbuch Allgemeine Psychologie (pp. 115–187). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1973). Scanning visual images: some structural implications. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 90–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: the resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lüer, G., Werner, S., & Lass, U. (1995). Repräsentationen analogen Wissens im Gedächtnis. In D. Dörner & E. van der Meer (Eds.), Das Gedächtnis. Probleme-Trends-Perspektiven (pp. 75–125). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge: The Technology Press & Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, M. (1992). Mentale Modelle von Städten. Wissenspsychologische Untersuchungen am Beispiel der Stadt Münster. Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructures of cognition. (Vol. 2: Psychological and biological models). Cambridge: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. (1992). Priming and constraints it places on theories of memory and retrieval. Psychological Review, 99, 650–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. (1994). Theories of priming: II. Types of primes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 507–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P., Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1984). The mental representation of knowledge acquired from maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 723–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. T., & Golledge, R. G. (Eds.). (1976). Environmental knowing: theories, research, and methods. Stroudsburg, P. A.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition. A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading. Visual word recognition (pp. 264–337).

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H., & Keefe, D. E. (1989). Semantic context effects on visual word processing: A hybride prospective-retrospective processing theory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 24, pp. 202–248). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 467–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nomura, A., Miike, H., & Koga, K. (1991). Field theory approach for determining optical flow. Pattern Recognition Letters, 12, 183–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Opwis, K., & Lüer, G. (1992). Modelle der Repräsentation von Wissen. In D. Albert & K. H. Stapf (Eds.), Gedächtnispsychologie: Erwerb, Nutzung und Speicherung von Informationen (Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Bereich C, Serie II, Bd.4) (pp. 337–431). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1991) (Ed.). Brain and space. Oxford. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 259–303). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1980). Mental imagery and the third dimension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 354–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1990). Wahrnehmung. In H. Spada (Ed.), Lehrbuch Allgemeine Psychologie (pp. 25–114). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981). The imagery debate: analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88, 16–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1994). Retrieving information from memory: Spreading-activation theories versus compound-cue theories. Psychological Review, 101, 177–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rehkämper, K. (1990). Mentale Bilder-analoge Repräsentation. In C. Habel & C. Freksa (Eds.), Repräsentation und Verarbeitung räumlichen Wissens (pp. 47–67). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehkämper, K. (1993). Picture yourself in a boat on a river-Über die Bildhaftigkeit mentaler Repräsentationen. Kognitionswissenschaft, 3, 117–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schade, U. (1992). Konnektionismus: Zur Modellierung der Sprachproduktion. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, K. (1997). Räumliche oder zeitliche Wissensorganisation? Zur mentalen Repräsentation der Blickpunktsequenz bei räumlichen Anordnungen. Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, K. (in prep.) Primingeffekte bei Routenwissen-eine Theorie zur Aktivationsausbreitung bei mentalen Raumrepräsentationen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, K., & Janzen, G. (1996). Zum Einfluß der Erwerbssituation auf die Raumkognition: Mentale Repräsentation der Blickpunktsequenz bei räumlichen Anordnungen. Sprache & Kognition, 15, 217–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, K., Janzen, G., & Herrmann, Th. (1995). Richtungseffekt oder serielles Lernen-ein Nachtrag (Arbeiten der Forschungsgruppe „Sprache und Kognition“, Bericht Nr. 55). Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, K., Katz, S., Janzen, G., & Herrmann, Th. (1997). Route knowledge and the effect of route direction: A comparison of priming and cued-recall (Arbeiten des Mannheimer Teilprojekts „Determinanten des Richtungseffekts“ im SPP Raumkognition, Bericht Nr. 2). Mannheim: Universität Mannheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shemyakin, F. N. (1962). Orientation in space. In B. G. Ananyev et al. (Ed.), Psychological science in the U.S.S.R. (Vol 1, pp. 186–255). Washington, DC: Department of Commerce, Office of Technical Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, A. W., & White, S. H. (1975). The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. In H. R. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 10–55). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tack, W. H. (1995). Repräsentationen menschlichen Wissens. In D. Dörner & E. van der Meer (Eds.), Das Gedächtnis. Probleme-Trends-Perspektiven (pp. 53–74). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trowbridge, C. C. (1913). Fundamental methods of orientation and imaginary maps. Science, 38, 888–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (1991). Spatial mental models. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 27, 109–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verri, A., & Poggio, T. (1989). Motion field and optical flow: Qualitative properties. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence., 11, 490–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagener-Wender, M. (1993). Mentale Repräsentation räumlicher Informationen. Bonn: Holos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagener-Wender, M., & Wender, K. F. (1990). Expectations, mental representation, and spatial inferences. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 25, pp. 137–157). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wender, K. F. & Wagener, M. (1990). Zur Verarbeitung räumlicher Informationen: Modelle und Experimente. Kognitionswissenschaft, 1, 4–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wippich, W. (1985). Lehrbuch der angewandten Gedächtnispsychologie. (Vol. 2). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D. (1993). Modalitätsspezifische Systeme der Repräsentation und Verarbeitung von Informationen: Überflüssige Gebilde, nützliche Fiktionen, notwendiges Übel oder zwangsläufige Folge optimierter Reizverarbeitung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 203–235.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schweizer, K., Herrmann, T., Janzen, G., Katz, S. (1998). The Route Direction Effect and its Constraints. In: Freksa, C., Habel, C., Wender, K.F. (eds) Spatial Cognition. Lecture Notes in Computer Science(), vol 1404. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-69342-4_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-69342-4_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-64603-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-540-69342-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics