Abstract
Intelligent behavior is characterized by flexible and creative pursuit of endogenously defined goals. It has emerged in humans through the stages of evolution that are manifested in the brains and behaviors of other vertebrates. Perception is a key concept by which to link brain dynamics to goal-directed behavior. This archetypal form of intentional behavior is an act of observation into time and space, by which information is sought to guide future action, and by which the perceiver modifies itself through learning from the sensory consequences of its own actions. Chaotic brain dynamics creates the goals, expresses them by means of behavioral actions, and defines the meaning of the requested information. These acts include the making of representations (e.g. numbers, words, graphs, sounds, gestures) for communication to other brains in validation and coordination of experience. The failure of artificial intelligence to achieve its stated aims can be attributed to taking too literally these man-made descriptive representations as the tokens of brain action, whereas in brains there is no information, only dynamic flows and operators.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Preview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Barrie, M., Freeman, W.J., Lenhart, M. (1996) Modulation by discriminative training of spatial patterns of gamma EEG amplitude and phase in neocortex of rabbits. Journal of Neurophysiology 76: 520–539.
Blum, L., Shub, M., Smale, S. (1989) On a theory of computation and complexity over the real numbers. Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society 21: 1–46.
Bures, J., Buresová, O., Krivánek, J. (1974) The Mechanism and Applications of Leão’s Spreading Depression of Electroencephalographic Activity. New York: Academic Press.
Chang, H.J., Freeman, W.J. (1996) Parameter optimization in models of the olfactory system. Neural Networks 9: 1–14.
Clark, A. (1996) Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
Clark, R.E., Squire, L.R. (1998) Classical conditioning and brain systems: The role of awareness. Science 280: 77–81.
Freeman, W.J. (1975) Mass Action in the Nervous System. New York: Academic Press.
Freeman, W.J. (1987) Simulation of chaotic EEG patterns with a dynamic model of the olfactory system. Biological Cybernetics 56: 139–150.
Freeman, W.J. (1988) Strange attractors that govern mammalian brain dynamics shown by trajectories of electroencephalographic (EEG) potential. IEEE Trans. Circuits & Systems 35: 781–783.
Freeman, W.J. (1991) The physiology of perception. Scientific American 264: 78–85.
Freeman, W.J. (1992) Tutorial in neurobiology: From single neurons to brain chaos. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 2: 451–482.
Freeman, W.J. (1995) Societies of Brains. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Freeman, W.J. (1996) Random activity at the microscopic neural level in cortex (“noise”) sustains and is regulated by low-dimensional dynamics of macroscopic cortical activity (“chaos”). International Journal of Neural Systems 7: 473–480.
Freeman, W.J. (2000) Neurodynamics: An Exploration in Mesoscopic Brain Dynamics. London: Springer Verlag.
Freeman, W.J. (2001) How Brains Make Up Their Minds. New York: Columbia University Press.
Freeman, W.J., Baird, B. (1989) Effects of applied electric current fields on cortical neural activity. In: E. Schwartz (Ed.) Computational Neuroscience (pp. 274–287). New York: Plenum Press.
Freeman, W.J., Chang, H.J., Burke, B.C., Rose, P.A., Badler, J. (1997) Taming chaos: Stabilization of aperiodic attractors by noise. IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems 44: 989–996.
Freeman, W.J., Schneider, W. (1982) Changes in spatial patterns of rabbit olfactory EEG with conditioning to odors. Psychophysiology 19: 44–56.
Goltz, F.L. (1892) Der Hund ohne Grosshirn. Siebente Abhandlung über die Verrichtungen des Grosshirns. Pflügers Archiv 51: 570–614.
Grajski, K.A., Freeman, W.J. (1989) Spatial EEG correlates of non-associative and associative learning in rabbits. Behavioral Neuroscience 103: 790–804.
Gray, C.M., Freeman, W.J. (1987) Induction and maintenance of epileptiform activity in the rabbit olfactory bulb depends on centrifugal input. Experimental Brain Research 68: 210–212.
Gray, C.M., Freeman W.J., Skinner, J.E. (1986) Chemical dependencies of learning in the rabbit olfactory bulb: acquisition of the transient spatial-pattern change depends on norepinephrine. Behavioral Neuroscience 100: 585–596.
Gray, C.M., Singer, W. (1989) Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 86: 1698–1702.
Hardcastle, V.G. (1994) Psychology’s binding problem and possible neurobiological solutions. Journal of Consciousness Studies 1: 66–90.
Helmholtz, H. von (1879/1925) Treatise on Physiological Optics: Vol. 3. The Perceptions of Vision (J.P.C. Southall, Trans.). Rochester NY: Optical Society of America.
Hendriks-Jansen, H. (1996) Catching ourselves in the act: Situated activity, interactive emergence, evolution, and human thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Herrick, C.J. (1948) The Brain of the Tiger Salamander. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Kaneko, K. (1986) Collapse of Tori and Genesis of Chaos in Dissipative Systems. Singapore: World Scientific.
Kay, L.M., Freeman, W.J. (1998) Bidirectional processing in the olfactory-limbic axis during olfactory behavior. Behavioral Neuroscience 112: 541–553.
Kay, L.M., Lancaster, L., Freeman, W.J. (1996) Reafference and attractors in the olfactory system during odor recognition. International Journal of Neural Systems 7: 489–496.
O’Keefe, J., Nadel, L. (1978) The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.
Ramón y Cajal, S. (1911) Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de l’Homme et des Vertèbres (Vols. I and II). Paris: Maloine.
Roth, G. (1987) Visual Behavior in Salamanders. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Shaw, G.L., Palm, G. (Eds.) (1988) Brain Theory. Reprint Volume. Singapore: World Scientific.
Singer, W., Gray, C.M. (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18: 555–586.
Sperry, R.W. (1958) Physiological plasticity and brain circuit theory. In: H.F. Harlow, C.N. Woolsey (Eds.) Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior (pp. 401–424). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.
Taga, G. (1994) Emergence of bipedal locomotion through entrainment among the neuro-musculo-skeletal system and the environment. Physica. D. 75: 190–208.
Tani, J. (1996) Model-based learning for mobile robot navigation from the dynamical systems perspective. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics 26B: 421–436.
von Hoist, E., Mittelstadt, H. (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkung zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften 37: 464476.
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2003 Springer-Verlag London Limited
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Freeman, W.J. (2003). Performance of Intelligent Systems Governed by Internally Generated Goals. In: Hecht-Nielsen, R., McKenna, T. (eds) Computational Models for Neuroscience. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-0085-0_3
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-0085-0_3
Publisher Name: Springer, London
Print ISBN: 978-1-85233-593-9
Online ISBN: 978-1-4471-0085-0
eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive