Abstract
In knowledge-work, there are increasing amounts of complex information rendered by information technology, which has led to the common term, information overload. Information visualization is one area where empirically tested semantic theory has not yet caught up with that of the underlying information storage and retrieval theory, contributing to information overload. In spite of a vast body of cognitive theory, much of the human factors research on information visualization has overlooked it. Specifically, information displays have facilitated the data gathering (ontological) aspects of human problem-solving and decision-making, but have exacerbated the meaning-making (epistemological) aspects of those activities by presenting information in linear rather than in graphical (holistic) forms. Drawing from extant empirical research, we present a thesis suggesting that cognitive load may be reduced when holistic information is imbued with transformational grammar to help alleviate the information overload problem, along with a methodological approach for investigation.
Preview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Killmer, K.A., Koppel, N.B.: So much information, so little time: Evaluating web resources with search engines. Technol. Horizons in Education Journal 30, 21–29 (2002)
Workman, M., Lesser, M.F., Kim, J.: An exploratory study of cognitive load in diagnosing patient conditions. International Journal for Quality in Health Care 19, 127–133 (2007)
Bennett, K.B., Flach, J.M: Graphical displays: Implications for divided attention, focused attention, and problem solving. Human Factors 34, 513–533 (1992)
Chechile, R.A, Eggleston, R.G., Fleischman, R.N., Sasseville, A.M.: Modeling the cognitive content of displays. Human Factors 31, 31–43 (1989)
Lohr, L.: Creating visuals for learning and performance: Lessons in visual literacy. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ (2003)
Dyer, C.: Doctors go on trial for manslaughter after removing wrong kidney. British Medical Journal 324, 10–11 (2002)
National Transportation and Safety Board. Collision of two Burlington Northern Santa Fe freight trains. Washington DC: NTSB Report PB2006-916302 Notation 7793A (2002)
CNN. Major power outage hits New York, other large cities, (Retrieved July 07, 2007) http://www.cnn.com/2003/US/08/14/power.outage
Bradshaw, L.: Information overload and the Hurricane Katrina post-disaster disaster. Information Enterprises, Fremantle, WA (2006)
Larkin, J.H., Simon, H.A.: Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousands words. Cognitive Science 11, 65–99 (1987)
Healey, C., Kellogg, G., Booth, S., Enns, J.T.: High-speed visual estimation using preattentive processing. ACM Trans. on Computer-Human Interaction 14, 107–135 (1996)
Chernoff, H.: Using faces to represent points in k dimensional space graphically. Journal of American Statistical Association 68, 361–368 (1973)
Kondo, H., Mori, H.: A computer system applying the face method to represent multiphasic tests. Medical Information 12, 217–222 (1987)
Morris, M.: Kiviat graphs - conventions and figures of merit. ACM SIGMETRICS Performance Evaluation Review 3, 2–8 (1974)
Kolence, K.W., Kiviat, P.J.: Software unit profiles and Kiviat figures. ACM SIGMETRICS Performance Evaluation Review 2, 2–12 (1973)
Pola, Pl., Cruccu, G., Dolce, G.: Star-like display of EEG spectral values. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 50, 527–529 (1980)
Marcus, A.: Dashboards in your future. Communications of the ACM 13, 48–60 (2006)
Posner, M.I.: Chronometric explorations of mind. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1978)
Chomsky, N.: Human language and other semiotic systems. Semiotica 25, 31–44 (1979)
Cooper, G.: Cognitive load theory as an aid for instructional design. Australian Journal of Educational Technology 6, 108–113 (1990)
Rehder, B., Hoffman, A.B.: Eye tracking and selective attention in category learning. Cognitive Psychology 51, 1–41 (2005)
Komlodi, A., Rheingans, P., Ayachit, U., Goodall, J.R., Joshi, A.: A user-centered look at glyph-based security visualization. IEEE Conference Workshop on Visualization for Computer Security 26, 21–28 (2005)
Mayer, R.E.: Multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2001)
Shneiderman, B.: Designing the user interface: Strategies for effective human-computer interaction. Addison-Wesley Longman Publishing Co, Boston (1992)
Tufte, E.R.: The visual display of quantitative information. Graphics Press, Cheshire, CT (1986)
Powsner, S.M., Tufte, E.R.: Graphical summary of patient status. The Lancet 344, 386–389 (1994)
Bederson, B.B., Shneiderman, B., Wattenberg, M.: Ordered and quantum treemaps: Making effective use of 2D space to display hierarchies. In: Bedderson, B.B., Shneiderman, B. (eds.) The craft of information visualization, pp. 257–278. Morgan Kaufmann Publishers, San Francisco (2002)
Bennett, K.B., Payne, M., Calcaterra, J., Nittoli, B.: An empirical comparison of alternative methodologies for the evaluation of configural displays. The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 42, 287–298 (2000)
Carswell, C.M., Wickens, C.D.: The proximity compatibility principle: Its psychological foundation and relevance to display design. Human Factors 37, 473–494 (1995)
Loft, S., Sanderson, P., Neal, A., Mooij, M.: Modeling and predicting mental workload in en route air traffic control: Critical review and broader implications. The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 49, 376–399 (2007)
Johnson, M.H.: The development of visual attention: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 735–750. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Rafal, R., Robertson, L.: The neurology of visual attention. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 625–648. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Schacter, D.L.: Implicit memory: New frontiers for cognitive neuroscience. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 824–825. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Tulving, E.: Working memory: An Introduction. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 751–754. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Caplan, D.: The cognitive neuroscience of syntactic processing. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 871–880. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Garrett, M.: The structure of language processing: Neuropsychological evidence. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 881–900. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Gavrilova, T.A., Voinov, A.V.: The cognitive approach to the creation of ontology. Nauchno-Tekhnicheskaya Informatsiya 2, 59–64 (2007)
Schroeder, J., Xu, J., Chen, H., Chau, M.: Automated criminal link analysis based on domain knowledge. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 58, 842–855 (2007)
McBride, B.: The resource description framework (RDF) and its vocabulary description language RDFS. In: Staab, S., Studer, R. (eds.) The handbook on ontologies in Information Systems, pp. 223–257. Springer, Heidelberg (2003)
Albers, M.J.: Information design considerations for improving situation awareness in complex problem solving. In: ACM SIG Design of Communication, Proc. of the 17th ann. Int. conference on computer documentation, New Orleans, LA, pp. 154–158. ACM Press, New York (1999)
Endsley, M.R., Bolte, B., Jones, D.G.: Designing for situation awareness: An approach to user-centered design. Taylor & Francis, NY (2003)
Sweller, J.: Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science 12, 257–285 (1988)
Brunken, R., Steinbacher, S., Plass, J.L., Leutner, D.: Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning using dual task methodology. Journal of Experimental Psychology 49, 109–119 (2002)
Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., Remington, R.W.: The role of input and output modality parings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology 52, 291–345 (2006)
Woods, D.D.: The cognitive engineering of problem representations. In: Weir, G.R.S., Alty, J.L. (eds.) Human-computer interaction and complex systems, pp. 169–188. Academic Press, London (1994)
Norman, D.A., Bobrow, D.J.: On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology 7, 44–64 (1975)
Richardson-Klavvehn, A., Gardiner, J.M., Ramponi, C.: Level of processing and the process-dissociation procedure: Elusiveness of null effects on estimates of automatic retrieval. Memory 10, 349–364 (2002)
Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G.J.: Working Memory. In: Bower, G. (ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, pp. 47–90. Academic Press, New York (1974)
Breitmeyer, B.G.: Visual masking: past accomplishments, present status, future developments. Advances in Psychology 3, 9–20 (2007)
Holzinger, A., Geierhofer, R., Errath, M.: Semantic information in medical information systems - From data and information to knowledge: Facing information overload. In: Proceedings of I-MEDIA 2007 and I-SEMANTICS 2007, Graz, Austria, pp. 323–330 (2007)
Monsell, S., Driver, J.: Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (2000)
Jacoby, L.L.: A process discrimination framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30, 531–541 (1991)
Barnhardt, T.M.: Number of solutions effects in stem decision: Support for the distinction between identification and production processes in priming. Memory 13, 725–748 (2005)
Halford, G.S., Baker, R., McCredden, J.E., Bain, J.D.: How many variables can humans process? Psychological Science 16, 70–76 (2005)
Smith, E.E., Jonides, J.: Working memory in humans: Neuropsychological evidence. In: Gazzanga, M.S. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 1009–1020. MIT Press, Cambridge MA (1995)
Anderson, J.R.: Cognitive psychology and its implications. Worth Publishers, New York, NY (2000)
Reder, L.M., Schunn, C.D.: Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory. In: Reder, L.M. (ed.) Implicit memory and metacognition, pp. 45–78. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1996)
Sternberg, R.J.: Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1977)
Cowan, N.: The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, 87–185 (2000)
Sweller, J.: Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science 12, 257–285 (1988)
Bourke, P.A., Duncan, J.: Effect of template complexity on visual search and dual-task performance. Psychological Science 16, 208–213 (2005)
Draycott, S.G., Kline, P.: Validation of the AGARD STRES battery of performance tests. Human Factors 38, 347–361 (1996)
Wise, J.A., Thomas, J.J, Pennock, K., Lantrip, D., Pottier, M., Schur, A., Crow, V.: Visualizing the non-visual: spatial analysis and interaction with information for text documents. In: Card, S., Mackinlay, J. (eds.) Readings in information visualization: Using vision to think, pp. 442–450. Morgan Kaufmann Publishers Inc., San Francisco (1999)
Langer, S.: Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism of reason, rite, and art. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA (1957)
Bergeron, V.: Anatomical and functional modularity in cognitive science: Shifting the focus. Philosophical Psychology 20, 175–195 (2007)
Miller, E.K., Chelazzi, L., Lueschow, A.: Multiple memory systems in the visual cortex. In: Gazzanga, G. (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, pp. 475–490. MIT Press, Cambridge (1995)
Simon, G., Petit, L., Bernard, C., Rebaï, M.: Occipito-temporal N170 ERPs could represent a logographic processing strategy in visual word recognition. Behavioral and Brain Functions 3, 3–21 (2007)
Legge, G.E., Gu, Y., Luebker, A.: Efficiency of graphical perception. Perception and Psychophysics 46, 365–374 (1989)
Tsang, M., Morris, N., Balakrishnan, R.: Temporal thumbnails: Rapid visualization of time-based viewing data. In: Proceedings of the 15th annual ACM symposium on user interface software and technology, pp. 175–178. ACM Press, New York (2002)
Montgomery, D.A.: Human sensitivity to variability information in detection decisions. Human Factors 41, 90–105 (1999)
Pollatesk, A., Reichle, E.D., Rayner, K.: Tests of the EZReader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye movement control. Cognitive Psychology 52, 1–56 (2006)
Shiffrin, R.M., Schneider, W.: Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review 84, 127–190 (1977)
Bransford, J.D., Franks, J.J.: The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2, 331–350 (1971)
Kozma, R.B.: Learning with media. Review of Educational Research 61, 179–211 (1991)
Trafton, G.J., Trickett, S.B.: Note-taking for self-explanation and problem solving. Human-Computer Interaction 16, 1–38 (2001)
Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., Blaser, E.: The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research 35, 1897–1916 (1995)
Lesser, M.F.: GIFIC: A graphical interface for information cognition for intensive care. In: Proceedings from the 18th Ann. Symp. on Computers in Applied Medical Care (1994)
Starren, J., Johnson, S.B.: An object-oriented taxonomy of medical data presentations. Journal of the American Medical Information Association 7, 1–20 (2000)
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Editor information
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2007 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg
About this paper
Cite this paper
Workman, M. (2007). Cognitive Load Research and Semantic Apprehension of Graphical Linguistics. In: Holzinger, A. (eds) HCI and Usability for Medicine and Health Care. USAB 2007. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 4799. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-76805-0_33
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-76805-0_33
Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg
Print ISBN: 978-3-540-76804-3
Online ISBN: 978-3-540-76805-0
eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)