Skip to main content
Log in

A neural model of the dynamic activation of memory

  • Published:
Biological Cybernetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We study an Attractor Neural Network that stores natural concepts, organized in semantic classes. The concepts are represented by distributed patterns over a space of attributes, and are related by both semantic and episodic associations. While semantic relations are expressed through an hierarchical coding over the attribute space, episodic links are realized via specific synaptic projections. Due to dynamic thresholds expressing neuronal fatigue, the network's behavior is characterized by convergence toward the concept patterns on a short time scale, and by transitions between the various patterns on a longer time scale. In its baseline, undamaged state, the network manifests semantic, episodic, and random transitions, and demonstrates the phenomen of priming. Modeling possible pathological changes, we have found that increasing the ‘noise’ level or the rate of neuronal fatigue decreases the frequency of semantic transitions. When neurons characterized by large synaptic connectivity are deleted, semantic transitions decay before the episodic ones, in accordance with the findings in patients with Alzheimer's disease.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbott LF (1990) Modulation of function and gated learning in a network memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 87:9241–9245

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams RD, Victor M (1989) Principles of neurology. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Amit DJ (1989) Modeling brain function: the world of attractor neural networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR (1976) Language, memory, and thought, Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson JR (1985) Cognitive psychology and its implications. W. H. Freeman and Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertoni-Freddari C, Fattoretti P, Casoli T, Meier-Ruge W, Ulrich J (1990) Morphological adaptive response of the synaptic junctional zones in the human dentate gyrus during aging and Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res 517:69–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Bok ST (1959) Histonomy of the cerebral cortex. Elsevier, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter GA, Grossberg S (1990) Art 3: Hierarchical search using chemical transmitters in self-organizing pattern recognition architectures. Nerual Networks 3:129–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins A, Loftus E (1975) A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol Rev 82:407–428

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins AM, Quillian MR (1969) Retrieval from semantic memory. J Verb Learn Behav 8:240–247

    Google Scholar 

  • DeKosky ST, Scheff SW (1990) Synapse loss in frontal loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer's diease: Correlation with cognitive severity. Ann Neurol 27(5):457–464

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman S (1981) Representing implicit knowledge. In: Hinton GE, Anderson JA (eds) Parallel models of associative memory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Feigelman MW, Ioffe LB (1987) The augmented models of associative memory asymmetric interaction and hierarchy of patterns. Int J Mod Phys B 1:51–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J (1990) Inferotemporal units in selective attention and short term memory. J Neurophysiol 64:681–697

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopolous AP, Kettner RE, Schwartz AB (1988) Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neural population. J Neurosci 8:2928–2937

    Google Scholar 

  • Granholm E, Butters N (1988) Associative encoding and retrieval in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. Brain-Cogn 7(3):335–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gröbler T, Marton P, Erdi P (1991) On the dynamic organization of memory. Biol Cybern 65:73–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutfreund H (1988) Neural networks with hierarchically correlated patterns. Phys Rev A 37:570–577

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasselmo ME, Bower JM (1992) Chlorinergic modulation of cortical memory function. J Neurophysiol 67:1230–1246

    Google Scholar 

  • Heit G, Smith ME, Halgren E (1988) Neural encoding of individual words and faces by the human hippocampus and amygdala. Nature 333:773–775

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann M, Tsodyks M.V. (1991) Pattern hierarchy destruction in nonlinear neural networks. Workshop on Neural Networks in Biology and High Energy Physics, Isola d'Elba, 1991

  • Hinton GE (1981) Implementing semantic networks in parallel hardware. In: Hinton GE, Anderson JA (eds) Parallel models of associative memory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman RE (1987) Schizophrenia-mania dichotomy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:178–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman RE, Dobscha S (1989) Schizophrenia Bull 15(3):477–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn D, Usher M (1989) Neural networks with dynamic thresholds. Phys Rev A 40:1036–1044

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn D, Usher M (1990) Excitatory-inhibitory networks with dynamical thresholds. Int J Neural Syst 1:249–257

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman BT, Van Hoesen GW, Damasio AR, Barnes CL (1984)Science 225:1168–1170

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom JF (1987) The cognitive unconscious. Science 237:1145–1152

    Google Scholar 

  • King R, Barchas JD, Huberman BA (1984) Chaotic behavior in dopamine neurodynamics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 81:1244–1247

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinfeld D (1986) Sequential state generation by models of neural networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83:9469–9473

    Google Scholar 

  • Mamelak AD, Hobson JA (1989) Dream bizarreness as the cognitive correlate of altered neuronal behavior in REM sleep. J Cogn Neurosci 1(3):201–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel AJ (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: an approach to the relations between phenomenal and perceptual processes. Cognit Psych 15:238–300

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller EK, Li L, Desdimone D (1991) A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science 254:1377–1379

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Petersen SE, Fox PT, Raichle ME (1988) Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240:1627–1631

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff R, McKoon G (1981) Does activation really spread? Psychol Rev 88:454–462

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritter H, Kohonen T (1989) Self-organizing semantic maps. Biol Cybern 61:241–254

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosh E, Mervis C (1975) Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories. Cogn Psych 7:573–605

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart DE, McClelland J (1986) Parallel distributed processing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakay K, Miyashita Y (1990) Neural organization of the long term memory of pair associates. Nature 354:152–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon DP, Shimamura AP, Butters N, Smith S (1988) Lexical and semantic priming deficits in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 10(4):477–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL (1989) Memory. In: Posner MI (eds) Foundations of ognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 683–708

    Google Scholar 

  • Servan-Schrieber D, Printz H, Cohen JD (1990) A network model of cathecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 249:892–895

    Google Scholar 

  • Skarda CA, Freeman WJ (1987) Does the brain make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behav Brain Sci 10:161–165

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith E, Shoeben E, Rips LJ (1974) Structure and process in semantic memory: a featural model for semantic decisions. Psych Rev 81:214–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky P (1986) Neural and conceptual interpretation of PDP models. In: Rumelhard DE, McClelland J (eds) Parallel distributed processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 390–431

    Google Scholar 

  • Sompolinsky H, Kanter I (1986) Temporal associations in assymetric networks. Phys Rev Lett 57:2861–2864

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire LR (1982) The neuropsychology of human memory. Ann Rev Neurosci 5:241–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton JP, Mamelak AN, Hobson JA (1992) Modeling states of waking and sleeping. Psychiatr Ann 22(2):1–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Swindale NV, Vital-Durand F, Blakemore C (1981) Recovery from monocular deprivation in the monkey. Reversal of anatomical effects in the visual cortex. Proc R Soc Lond Sec B. 213:435–450

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka K, Saito Y (1991) Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemproal cortex of macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 66:170–189

    Google Scholar 

  • Terry RD, Peck A, Teresa R, Schechter R, Hyoroupian DS (1981)Ann Neurol 10:184–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman A, Gelade G (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12:97–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsodyks MV (1990) Hierarchical associative memory in neural networks with low activity level. Mod Phys Lett B 4:259–265

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (1985) How many memory systems are there? Am Psychol 40:385–398

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Herrmann, M., Ruppin, E. & Usher, M. A neural model of the dynamic activation of memory. Biol. Cybern. 68, 455–463 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00198778

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00198778

Keywords

Navigation