Skip to main content

Origin and nature of cognitive and systematic musicology: An introduction

  • I.Gestalt Theory Revisited
  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Music, Gestalt, and Computing (JIC 1996)

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Computer Science ((LNAI,volume 1317))

Included in the following conference series:

Abstract

This paper gives an introduction to the origin and nature of cognitive and systematic musicology. A brief overview of the history of systematic musicology is presented and the developments are sketched that contributed to the rise of cognitive musicology. It is argued that the breakthrough of computing has offered many new opportunities to revisit the old problems of Gestalt perception and its associated theory. The paper closes with a perspective on future developments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allport, F. (1967). Theories of perception and the concept of structure. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apostel, L. (Ed.). (1972). De eenheid van de cultuur: Naar een algemene systeemtheorie als instrument van de eenheid van ons kennen en ons handelen. Boom: Meppel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. (1995). Schema theory. In M. Arbib (Ed.), The handbook of brain theory and neural networks. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balaban, M., Ebcioglu, K., & Laske, O. (Eds.). (1992). Understanding music with AI: Perspectives on music cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boretz, B. (1969a). Meta-variations II: Sketch of a musical system. Perspectives of New Music, 8, 49–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boretz, B. (1969b). Meta-variations I: Studies in the foundations of musical thought. Perspectives of New Music, 8, 1–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bregman, A. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broeckx, J. (1975). Contemporary values on musical style and aesthetics. Antwerp: Metropolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broeckx, J. (1981). Muziek, ratio en affect: Over de wisselwerking van rationeel denken en affectief beleven bij voortbrengst en ontvangst van muziek. Antwerp: Metropolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camurri, A. (1990). On the role of artificial intelligence in music research. Interface — Journal of New Music Research, 19, 219–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camurri, A., Canepa, C., Frixione, M., & Zaccaria, R. (1992). HARP: A framework and a system for intelligent composer's assistance. In D. Baggi (Ed.), Readings in computer generated music. Los Almitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camurri, A., Frixione, M., & Innocenti, C. (1994). A cognitive model and a knowledge representation system for music and multimedia. Journal of New Music Research, 23, 317–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camurri, A., & Leman, M. (1997). AI-based music signal applications. In C. Roads, G. De Poli, S. Pope, & A. Piccialli (Eds.), Musical signal processing.Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chailley, J. (1967). Expliquer l'harmonie. Paris: Editions Rencontre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosi, P., De Poli, G., & Lauzzana, G. (1994). Auditory modelling and selforganizing neural networks for timbre classification. Journal of New Music Research, 23, 71–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlhaus, C. (Ed.). (1971). Einführung in die systematische Musikwissenschaft. Cologne: A. Volk.

    Google Scholar 

  • de la Motte-Haber, H. (1982). Umfang, Methode und Ziel der Systematischen Musikwissenschaft. In C. Dahlhaus & H. de la Motte-Haber (Eds.), Systematische Musikwissenschaft. Wiesbaden: Akademische Verlangsgesellschaft Athenaion.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Poli, G., Piccialli, A., & Roads, C. (Eds.). (1991). Representations of musical signals. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elschek, O. (1992). Die Musikforschung der Gegenwart (Vols. 1, 2). Vienna: Stiglmayr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ertel, S., Kemmler, L., & Stadler, M. (Eds.). (1975). Gestalttheorie in der modernen Psychologie. Darmstadt: Steinkopff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faltin, P., & Reinecke, H. (Eds.). (1973). Musik und Verstehen: Aufsätze zur semiotischen Theorie, ästhetik und Soziologie der musikalischen Rezeption. Cologne: A. Volk, H. Gerig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Födermayr, F. (1983). Zum Konzept einer vergleichend-systematischen Musikwissenschaft. Musikethnologische Sammelbände, 6, 25–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Födermayr, F., & Deutsch, W. (1992). Musik als Geistes-und Naturwissenschaftliches Problem. In W. Gratzer & A. Lindmayr (Eds.), De Editione Musices — Festschrift Gerhard Croll zum 65. Geburtstag. Laaber: Laaber-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1957). Psychologie du temps. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1974). Psychologie du rythme. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frances, R. (1958). La perception de la musique. Paris: Vrin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1987). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, W. (1980). Vergleichende Musikwissenschaft: Ausgewählte Aufsätze. Vienna, Föhrenau: Elisabeth Stiglmayr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey, J. (1977). Multidimensional perceptual scaling of musical timbres. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 61, 1270–1277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1933). The fundamental propositions of Gestalt psychology. Psychological Review, 40, 13–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hesse, H. (1972). Die Wahrnehmung von Tonhöhe und Klangfarbe als Problem der Hörtheorie. Cologne: A. Volk.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hostinský, O. (1879). Die lehre von den musikalischen klängen. Prag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karbusicky, V. (1986). Grundrißder musikalischen Semantik. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klix, F. (1971). Information und Verhalten. Bern, Stuttgart: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, W. (1909). Experimental-Untersuchungen zur Lehre vom Rhythmus. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 52, 1–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1910a). Akustische Untersuchungen 1. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 54, 241–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1910b). Akustische Untersuchungen 2. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 58, 59–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1913). Akustische Untersuchungen 3a. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 64, 92–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1915). Akustische Untersuchungen 3b. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 72, 1–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristof, W. (1969). Untersuchungen zur Theorie psychologischen Messens. Meisenheim am Glan: A. Hain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl, C. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl, C., & Kessler, E. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89, 334–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl, C., & Shepard, R. N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 579–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubik, G. (1973). Verstehen in afrikanischen Musikkulturen. In H. Reinecke & P. Faltin (Eds.), Musik und Verstehen. Cologne: A. Volk, H. Gerig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubik, G. (1983). Kognitive Grundlagen afrikanischer Musik. In A. Simon (Ed.), Musik in Afrika. Berlin: Museum für Völkerkunde.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunst, J. (1976). Making sense in music 1: The use of mathematical logic. Interface — Journal of New Music Research, 5, 3–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laske, O. (1977). Music, memory and thought. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Microfilms International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laske, O. (Ed.). (1986). Cognitive musicology. Ghent. (Special issue of CCAI — Journal for the integrated study of artificial intelligence, cognitive science, and applied epistemology, Vol. 3, No. 3)

    Google Scholar 

  • Laske, O. (1988). Can we formalize and program musical knowledge? An inquiry into the focus and scope of cognitive musicology. In M. Boroda (Ed.), Musikometrika I. Bochum: Studienverlag Dr. N. Brockmeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leman, M. (1989). Symbolic and subsymbolic information processing in models of musical communication and cognition. Interface — Journal of New Music Research, 18, 141–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leman, M. (1993). Symbolic and subsymbolic description of music. In G. Haus (Ed.), Music processing. Madison, WI: Oxford University Press, A-R Editions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leman, M. (Ed.). (1994a). Auditory models in music research. Part II. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. (Special issue of Journal of New Music Research)

    Google Scholar 

  • Leman, M. (Ed.). (1994b). Auditory models in music research. Part I. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. (Special issue of Journal of New Music Research)

    Google Scholar 

  • Leman, M. (1995). Music and schema theory: Cognitive foundations of systematic musicology. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, P., & Norman, D. (1977). Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longuet-Higgins, C. (1987). Mental processes: Studies in cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundin, R. (1967). An objective psychology of music. New York, NY: The Ronald Press. (Originally published in 1953)

    Google Scholar 

  • Marothy, J. (1974). Music and the bourgeois, music and the proletarian. Budapest: Akad. Kiado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, M. (1969). The technology of computer music. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, M., & Pierce, J. (Eds.). (1991). Current directions in computer music research. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, S., & Bregman, A. S. (1979). Hearing musical streams. Computer Music Journal, 8, 26–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. (1960). Plans and structure of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moisala, P. (1995). Cognitive study of music as culture: Basic premises for cognitive ethnomusicology. Journal of New Music Research, 24, 8–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monelle, R. (1992). Linguistics and semiotics in music. Chur: Harwood Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, B. C. (1989). An introduction to the psychology of hearing. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nattiez, J. (1975). Fondements d'une sémiologie de la musique. Paris: Union Générale d'Editions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Appleton-CenturiCrofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1982). The knowledge level. Artificial Intelligence, 18, 87–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: A psychoacoustical approach. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomp, R. (1976). Aspects of tone sensation. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. (1963). Operationism as methodology. Behavioral Sciences, 8, 234–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, R., & Reinecke, H. (Eds.). (1977). Zeichenprozesse: Semiotische Forschung in der Einzelwissenschaften. Wiesbaden: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft Athenaion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povall, R. (1995). Compositional methods in interactive performance environments. Journal of New Music Research, 109–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riemann, H. (1914/15). Ideen zu einer “Lehre von den Tonvorstellungen”. In Jahrbuch Peters (Vol. 21/22). Peters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riemann, H. (1916). Ideen zu einer “Lehre von den Tonvorstellungen”. In Jahrbuch Peters (Vol. 23). Peters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risset, J. (1978). Hauteur et timbre des sons (Tech. Rep.). Paris: Centre Georges Pompidou.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risset, J. (1992). The computer as an interface: Interlacing instruments and computer sounds; real-time and delayed synthesis; digital synthesis and processing; composition and performance. Interface — Journal of New Music Research, 21, 9–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risset, J., & Wessel, D. (1982). Exploration of timbre by analysis and synthesis. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roads, C., De Poli, G., & Pope, S. (1997). Musical signal processing. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., & Palmer, S. (1990). The legacy of Gestalt psychology. Scientific American, 268, 48–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, R. (1993). Interactive music systems. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruwet, N. (1975). Théorie et méthodes dans les études musicales: quelques remarques rétrospectiveset prélimina. Musique en Jeu, 17, 11–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabbe, H. (1977). Het muzikaal serialisme als techniek en als denkmethode. Ghent: Rijksuniversiteit Gent, Faculteit Letteren en Wijsbegeerte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandig, H. (1939). Beobachtungen an Zweiklängen in getrenntohriger und beidohriger Darbietung: Ein Beitrag zur Theorie der Konsonanz. In Neue psychologische Studien (Vol. XIV). Munich: Beck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, P. (1966). Traité des objets musicaux: Essai interdisciplines. Paris: Editions du Seuil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer, E. (1994). Psychoneural isomorphism: Historical background and current relevance. Philosophical Psychology, 7, 183–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, A. (1993a). Musikwissenschaft in der Emigration: Zur Vertreibung von Gelehrten und den Auswirkungen auf das Fach. In H. Heister, C. Maurer-Zenck, & P. Petersen (Eds.), Musik und Musiker im Exil: Folgen des Nazismus für die internationale Musikkultur. Frankfurt/M.: S. Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, A. (1993b). Systematische Musikwissenschaft: Traditionen, Ansätze, Aufgaben. Systematische Musikwissenschaft, 1, 145–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, A. (1993c). Zur Situation der Systematischen Musikwissenschaft. Systematische Musikwissenschaft, 1, 73–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seeger, C. (1977). Studies in musicology 1935–1975. Los Angeles, Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seifert, U. (1991). The schema concept: A critical review of its development and current use in cognitive science and research on music perception. In A. Cammuri & C. Canepa (Eds.), Proceedings of the IXth Colloquium on Musical Informatics. Genova: AIMI/DIST.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seifert, U. (1993). Systematische Musikwissenschaft als Grundlagenforschung der Musik. Systematische Musikwissenschaft, 1, 195–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, J. (1996). Human brain mapping. In R. Pratt & R. Spintge (Eds.), Music medicine, Vol.2. St. Louis: MMB.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. (1982). Structural representations of musical pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. (1989). Internal representations of universal regularities: A challenge for connectionism. In L. Nadel, L. Cooper, P. Culicover, & R. Harnish (Eds.), Neural connections, mental computation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stefani, G. (1976). Analisi, semiosi, semiotica. Rivista Ital. di Musicologia, 11, 106–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stefani, G. (1987). A theory of musical competence. Semiotica, 66, 7–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straub, W. (1929). Tonqualität und Tonhöhe. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 69, 289–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terhardt, E. (1974). Pitch, consonance, and harmony. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55, 1061–1069.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A., & Mazziotta, J. (Eds.). (1996). Brain mapping. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toiviainen, P. (1996). Optimizing auditory images and distance metrics for self-organizing timbre maps. Journal of New Music Research, 25, 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toiviainen, P., Kaipainen, M., & Louhivuori, J. (1995). Musical timbre: Similarity ratings correlate with computational feature space distances. Journal of New Music Research, 24, 282–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Noorden, L. (1975).Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Technische Hogeschool, Eindhoven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. (1935). Auditory perception. British Journal of Psychology, 25, 265–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. (1863/1968). Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage für die Theorie der Musik. Hildesheim: Georg Olms Verlagsbuchhandlung.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hornbostel, E. (1926). Psychologie der Gehörserscheinungen. In A. Bethe (Ed.), Handbuch der normalen und pathologischen Physiologie (Vols. XI, 1). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, U. (1993). Cognitive aspects of Amadinda xylophone music from Buganda: Inherent patterns reonsidered. Ethnomusicology, 37, 201–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weld, H. (1912). An experimental study of musical enjoyment. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 245–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellek, A. (1934). Der Raum in der Musik. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 91, 395–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellek, A. (1963). Musikpsychologie und Musikästhetik. Frankfurt/M.: Akad. Verlagsanstalt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wottawa, H. (1990). Einige Überlegungen zu (Fehl)Entwicklungen der psychologischen Methodenlehre. Psychologische Rundschau, 41, 84–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwicker, E., & Fastl, H. (1990). Psychoacoustics: Facts and models. Berlin,Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Marc Leman

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Leman, M., Schneider, A. (1997). Origin and nature of cognitive and systematic musicology: An introduction. In: Leman, M. (eds) Music, Gestalt, and Computing. JIC 1996. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 1317. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/BFb0034104

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BFb0034104

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-63526-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-540-69591-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics