Abstract
In the present communication, we have developed a computational model related to the conception of positional coding via centers-of-masses (centroids) of the objects’ luminance distributions. The model predictions have been tested by the results of our psychophysical study of geometrical illusion of extent evoked by a modified Brentano figure consisting of three separate spots clusters. In experiments, the centroids of the clusters were manipulated by varying the positions of additional non-target spots flanking the stimulus terminators. A good correspondence between the model predictions and the illusion magnitude changes provided convincing evidences in favor of “centroid” explanation of origin of the illusion investigated.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Badcock DR, Hess RF, Dobbins K (1996) Localization of element clusters: multiple cues. Vis Res 36: 1467–1472
Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vis Res 9: 1247–1258
Bertulis A, Bulatov A, Bielevicius A (2008) Influence of distracter on perceived stimulus length and angle size. Psichologija (Vilnius) 38: 29–39
Bettencourt KC, Somers DC (2009) Effects of target enhancement and distractor suppression on multiple object tracking capacity. J Vis 7: 1–11
Binsted G, Elliot D (1999) The Muller–Lyer illusion as a perturbation to the saccadic system. Hum Mov Sci 18: 103–117
Bocheva N, Mitrani L (1993) Model for visual localization. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 53: 377–384
Bulatov A, Bertulis A (2005a) Superimposition of illusory patterns with contrast variations. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 65: 51–60
Bulatov A, Bertulis A (2005b) Distracting effect in length matching. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 65: 265–269
Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Mickiene L (1997) Geometrical illusions: study and modelling. Biol Cybern 77: 395–406
Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Bielevicius A, Bulatova N (2009a) Interpretation of illusions of extent based on the centroid conception. Sens Syst (in Russian) 23(1): 3–12
Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Bielevicius A, Loginovich Y (2009b) Distracter influence on the right angle perception. Zh Vyss Nerv Deiat Pavlova (in Russian) 59(3): 259–268
Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Bulatova N, Loginovich Y (2009c) Centroid extraction and illusions of extent with different contextual flanks. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 69: 504–525
Coren S (1986) An efferent component in the visual perception of direction and extent. Physiol Rev 93: 391–410
Coren S, Girgus JS (1978) Seeing is deceiving: the psychology of visual illusions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ
DeLucia PR, Longmire SP, Kennish J (1994) Diamond-winged variants of the Muller–Lyer figure: a test of Virsu’s (1971) centroid theory. Percept Psychophys 55: 287–295
Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann Rev Neurosci 18: 193–222
Fermüller C, Malm H (2004) Uncertainty in visual processes predicts geometrical optical illusions. Vis Res 44: 727–749
Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH, Toni I, Zilles K (2002) Task instructions influence the cognitive strategies involved in line bisection judgments: evidence from modulated neural mechanisms revealed by fMRI. Neuropsychologia 40: 119–130
Gandhi SP, Heeger DJ, Boynton GM (1999) Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 16: 3314–3319
Glister R, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP (2010) The Müller–Lyer illusion: investigation of a center of gravity effect on the amplitudes of saccades. J Vis 10: 1–13
Hirsch J, Mjolsness E (1992) A center-of-mass computation describes the precision of random dot displacement discrimination. Vis Res 32: 335–346
Intriligator J, Cavanagh P (2001) The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cogn Psychol 43: 171–216
Kelly SP, Gomez-Ramirez M, Foxe JJ (2008) Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 18: 2629–2636
Levi DM, Klein SA, Aitsebaomo AP (1985) Vernier acuity, crowding, and cortical magnification. Vis Res 25: 963–977
Morgan MJ (1999) The Poggendorf illusion: a bias in the estimation of the orientation of virtual lines by second-stage filters. Vis Res 39: 2361–2380
Morgan MJ, Aiba TS (1985) Positional acuity with chromatic stimuli. Vis Res 25: 689–695
Morgan MJ, Glennerster A (1991) Efficiency of locating centers of dot-clusters by human observers. Vis Res 31: 2075–2083
Morgan MJ, Hole GJ, Glennerster A (1990) Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions. Vis Res 30: 1793–1810
Nakayama K, Mackaben M (1989) Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis Res 29: 1631–1647
Olzak L, Thomas J (1984) Seeing spatial patterns. In: Boff K, Kaufman L, Thomas J (eds) Handbook of perception and human performance, vol 1. Wiley, West Sussex, pp 7–27
Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Ann Rev Neurosci 13: 25–42
Post RB, Welch RB, Caufield K (1998) Relative spatial expansion and contraction within the Müller–Lyer and Judd illusions. Perception 27: 827–838
Proffitt DR, Cutting J (1980) Perceiving the centroid of curvilinear bounded rolling shapes. Percept Psychophys 28: 484–487
Restle F, Decker J (1977) Size of the Müller–Lyer illusion as a function of its dimensions: theory and data. Percept Psychophys 21: 489–503
Robinson DL, Petersen SE (1992) The pulvinar and visual salience. Trends Neurosci 15: 127–132
Sagi D, Julesz B (1986) Enhanced detection in the aperture of focal attention during simple shape discrimination tasks. Nature 321: 693–695
Searleman A, Porac C, Brzuszkiewicz L (2003) Changing the strength of the horizontal/vertical illusion by altering the placement of the functional fovea. Poster presented at the Eastern Psychological Association, Baltimore, MD
Searleman A, Porac C, Sherman M (2004) Manipulating the strength of the Ponzo illusion by controlling the position of the functional fovea. Poster presented at the Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, DC
Searleman A, Porac C, Dafoe C, Hetzel B (2005) Altering Muller–Lyer illusion magnitude using figural additions at the wing-shaft intersections. Am J Psychol 118: 619–637
Seizova-Cajic T, Gillam B (2006) Biases in judgments of separation and orientation elements belonging to different clusters. Vis Res 46: 2525–2534
Sierra-Vázquez V, Serano-Pedraza I (2007) Single-band amplitude demodulation of Müller–Lyer illusion images. Spanish J Psychol 10: 3–19
Solomon JA, Felisberti FM, Morgan MJ (2004) Crowding and the tilt illusion: toward a unified account. J Vis 4: 500–508
Somers DC, Dale AM, Seiffert AE, Tootell RBH (1999) Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. Neurobiology 96: 1663–1668
Toet A, Levi DM (1992) The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vis Res 32: 1349–1357
Troost BT, Weber RB, Daroff RB (1974) Hypometric saccades. Am J Ophthalm 78: 1002–1005
Virsu V (1971) Tendencies to eye movement, and misperception of curvature, direction, and length. Percept Psychophys 9: 65–72
Ward R, Casco C, Watt RJ (1985) The location of noisy visual stimuli. Can J Psychol 39: 387–399
Welch RB, Post RB, Lum W, Prinzmetal W (2004) The relationship between perceived length and egocentric location in Muller–Lyer figures with one versus two chevrons. Percept Psychophys 66: 1095–1104
Whitaker D, Walker H (1988) Centroid evaluation in the vernier alignment of random dot clusters. Vis Res 28: 777–784
Worrall N, Firth D (1974) The components of the standard and reverse Müller–Lyer illusions. Q J Exp Psychol 26: 342–354
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Bulatov, A., Bertulis, A., Gutauskas, A. et al. Center-of-mass alterations and visual illusion of extent. Biol Cybern 102, 475–487 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-010-0379-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-010-0379-5