Skip to main content
Log in

The distinction between first-person perspective and third-person perspective in virtual bodily self-consciousness

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Virtual Reality Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The distinction between the first-person perspective (1PP) and the third-person perspective (3PP) has been widely regarded as fundamental and rigid, and many researchers hold that genuine bodily illusions can only be experienced from the 1PP. We applied VR technology to investigate whether this mainstream view is correct. In our experiments, the participants were immersed in a VR environment in which they saw a life-sized virtual body either from the 1PP or from the 3PP. They either passively received tactile stimulations and/or actively interacted with a virtual soccer ball. Our VR system created novel visuo-motor-tactile correlations between the real and the virtual world: when the participant interacted with a real plastic soccer ball, he/she would feel corresponding tactile sensations and see the avatar performing the exact same movements. We found that a clear sense of ownership over the avatar was induced not only in the 1PP condition but also in the Passive-3PP and the Active-3PP conditions. We also observed evidence suggesting that it is possible to experience one’s body-location, 1PP-location, as well as self-location, both from the 1PP and from the 3PP. Together, we demonstrate that there is in fact no fundamental gap between embodied 1PP and embodied 3PP in the virtual world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information file.

References

  • Armel KC, Ramachandran VS (2003) Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc Biol Sci 270(1523):1499–1506

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aspell JE, Lenggenhager B, Blanke O (2009) Keeping in touch with one’s self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness. PLoS ONE 4(8):e6488

    ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aspell JE, Heydrich L, Marillier G, Lavanchy T, Herbelin B, Blanke O (2013) Turning body and self inside out: visualized heartbeats alter bodily self-consciousness and tactile perception. Psychol Sci 24(12):2445–2453

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker LR (2000) Persons and bodies: a constitution view. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull 107:238–246

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergström I, Kilteni K, Slater M (2016) First-person perspective virtual body posture influences stress: a virtual reality body ownership study. PLoS ONE 11(2):e0148060

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke O (2012) Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:556–571. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3292

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke O, Metzinger T (2009) Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn Sci 13:7–13

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke O, Mohr C (2005) Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin: implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness. Brain Res Rev 50:184–199

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke O, Slater M, Serino A (2015) Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness. Neuron 88(1):145–166

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS (eds) Test structural equation models. Sage, Newbury Park, CA, pp 136–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler AA, Héroux ME, Ganderia SC (2017) Body ownership and a new proprioceptive role for muscle spindles. Acta Physiol 220:19–27

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cabrera-Álvarez MJ, Clayton NS (2020) Neural processes underlying tool use in humans, macaques, and corvids. Front Psychol 11:560669

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carmines EG, McIver JP (1981) Analyzing models with unobserved variables: Analysis of covariance structures. In: Bohrnstedt W, Borgatta EF (eds) Social measurement: current issues. Sage, Newbury Park, CA, pp 65–115

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen WY, Huang HC, Lee YT, Liang C (2018) Body ownership and the four-hand illusion. Sci Rep 8:2153

    ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff K, Cosmelli D, Legrand D, Thompson E (2011) Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci 15:104–112

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1969) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge Academic, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • David N, Bewernick BH, Cohen MX, Newen AN, Lux S, Fink GR et al (2006) Neural representations of self versus other: visual-spatial perspective taking and agency in a virtual ball-tossing game. J Cogn Neurosci 18:898–910

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Day B, Ebrahimi E, Hartman LS, Pagano CC, Robb AC, Babu SV (2019) Examining the effects of altered avatars on perception-action in virtual reality. J Exp Psychol Appl 25(1):1–24

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Vignemont F (2018) Mind the body: an exploration of bodily self-awareness. Oxford University Press, Oxford, GB

    Google Scholar 

  • Debarba HG, Bovet S, Salomon R, Blanke O, Herbelin B, Boulic R (2017) Characterizing first and third person viewpoints and their alternation for embodied interaction in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 12:e0190109

    Google Scholar 

  • Dummer T, Picot-Annand A, Neal T, Moore C (2009) Movement and the rubber hand illusion. Perception 38:271–280

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebrahimi E, Robb A, Hartman LS, Pagano CC, Babu SV (2018) Effects of anthropomorphic fidelity of self-avatars on reach boundary estimation in immersive virtual environments. In: Proceedings of the 15th ACM symposium on applied perception (SAP '18), Association for Computing Machinery, New York, NY, USA, Article 2, pp 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1145/3225153.3225170

  • Ehrsson HH (2007) The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science 317(5841):1048

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson HH (2012) The Concept of body ownership and its relation to multisensory integration. In: Stein BE (ed) The new handbook of multisensory processing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cogn Sci 4(1):14–21

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2012) First-person perspective and immunity to error through misidentification. In: Miguens S, Preyer G (eds) Consciousness and subjectivity. Philosophical Analysis Ontos Publisher, Frankfurt, pp 187–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallivan JP, McLean DA, Valyear KF, Culham JC (2013) Decoding the neural mechanisms of human tool use. eLife 2:e00425

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez-Franco M, Cohn B, Ofek E, Burin D, Maselli A (2020) The self-avatar follower effect in virtual reality. In: 2020 IEEE conference on virtual reality and 3D user interfaces (VR). Atlanta, GA, USA, pp 18–25

  • Guterstam A, Ehrsson HH (2012) Disowning one’s seen real body during an out-of-body illusion. Conscious Cogn 21:1037–1042

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guterstam A, Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH (2011) The illusion of owning a third arm. PLoS ONE 6(2):e17208

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guterstam A, Björnsdotter M, Gentile G, Ehrsson HH (2015) Posterior cingulate cortex integrates the senses of self-location and body ownership. Curr Biol 25(11):1416–1425

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heydrich L, Dodds TJ, Aspell JE, Herbelin B, Bülthoff HH, Mohler BJ, Blanke O (2013) Visual capture and the experience of having two bodies–evidence from two different virtual reality techniques. Front Psychol 4:946

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hu L, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model 6:1–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang HC, Lee YT, Chen WY, Liang C (2017) The sense of 1PP-location contributes to shaping the perceived self-location together with the sense of body-location. Front Psychol 8:370. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00370

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl E (1973) Husserliana 14: zur phänomenologie der intersubjektivität: texte aus dem nachlass, zweiter teil: 1921–1928. Martinus Nijhoff, Den Haag

    Google Scholar 

  • Ionta S, Heydrich L, Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Fornari E, Chapuis D et al (2011) Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron 70:363–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkinson PM, Preston C (2015) New reflections on agency and body ownership: the moving rubber hand illusion in the mirror. Conscious Cogn 33:432–442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.020

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalckert A, Ehrsson HH (2012) Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: a dissociation of ownership and agency. Front Hum Neurosci 6:40

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kokkinara E, Kilteni K, Blom KJ, Slater M (2016) First person perspective of seated participants over a walking virtual body leads to illusory agency over the walking. Sci Rep 6:28879

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Legrand D (2007) Pre-reflective self-as-subject from experiential and empirical perspectives. Conscious Cogn 16:583–599

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legrand D, Ruby P (2009) What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychol Rev 116:252–282

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O (2007) Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science 317:1096–1099

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Blanke O (2009) Spatial aspects of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 18(1):110–117

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine J (2001) Purple haze: the puzzle of consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang C (2016) Self-as-subject and experiential ownership. In: Metzinger T, Windt JM (eds) Open MIND: philosophy and the mind sciences in the 21st century, vol 2. The MIT Press, London, pp 957–975

    Google Scholar 

  • Maselli A, Slater M (2013) The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion. Front Hum Neurosci 7:83. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00083

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maselli A, Slater M (2014) Sliding perspectives: dissociating ownership from self-location during full body illusions in virtual reality. Front Hum Neurosci 8:693. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00693

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger T (2003) Being no one. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller LE, Farnè A (2023) Tool use. In: Alsmith AJT, Longo MR (eds) The routledge handbook of bodily awareness. Taylor & Francis, Routledge, p 22

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel T (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philos Rev 83:435–450

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakul E, Orlando-Dessaints N, Lenggenhager B, Lopez C (2020) Measuring perceived self-location in virtual reality. Sci Rep 10:6802. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63643-y

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Palluel E, Aspell JE, Blanke O (2011) Leg muscle vibration modulates bodily self-consciousness: integration of proprioceptive, visual, and tactile signals. J Neurophysiol 105(5):2239–2247

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palluel E, Aspell JE, Lavanchy T, Blanke O (2012) Experimental changes in bodily self-consciousness are tuned to the frequency sensitivity of proprioceptive fibres. Neuroreport 23(6):354–359

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH (2008) If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS ONE 3:e3832

    ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Petkova VI, Khoshnevis M, Ehrsson HH (2011) The perspective matters! Multisensory integration in ego-centric reference frames determines full-body ownership. Front Psychol 2:35

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Riemer M, Kleinbohl D, Holzl R, Trojan J (2013) Action and perception in the rubber hand illusion. Exp Brain Res 229:383–393

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serino A, Alsmith A, Costantini M, Mandrigin A, Tajadura-Jimenez A, Lopez C (2013) Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 22:1239–1252

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slater M, Spanlang B, Sanchez-Vives MV, Blanke O (2010) First person experience of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 5:e10564–e10564

    ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M (2010) My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 48:703–712

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M, Haggard P (2005) The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:80–91

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M, Prabhu G, Haggard P (2006) Having a body versus moving your body: how agency structures body-ownership. Conscious Cogn 15:423–432

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7:38–42

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley K, May M, Ritzl A, Falkai P, Zilles K, Fink GR (2004) Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J Cogn Neurosci 16:817–827

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh LD, Moseley GL, Taylor JL, Gandevia SC (2011) Proprioceptive signals contribute to the sense of body ownership. J Physiol 589:3009–3021

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1958) The blue and brown books: preliminary studies for the “philosophical investigations.” Harper & Row, Oxford, England

    Google Scholar 

  • Yee N, Bailenson J (2007) The proteus effect: the effect of transformed self-representation on behavior. Hum Commun Res 33(3):271–290

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi D (2005) Subjectivity and selfhood: investigating the first-person perspective. Bradford Book/MIT Press, Cambridge MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi D (2020) Consciousness and selfhood: Getting clearer on for-me-ness and mineness. In: Kriegel U (ed) The oxford handbook of the philosophy of consciousness. Oxford University Press, London, pp 635–653

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Yun-Yu He, Chi-Hong Chen, Yi-Jan Wang, Tai-Yuan Chang, Hsiang-Yin Wu, Shang-Yi Chuang and Ssu-Ching Huang for their assistance in our lab. We would also like to thank Professor Chen-Gia Tsai from the Graduate Institute of Musicology for the SCR equipment. Yen-Tung Lee is funded by 2023 National Science and Technology Council Taiwanese Overseas Pioneers Grants (TOP Grants) for PhD Candidates. Finally, this study was supported by National Science and Technology Council of Taiwan (project: NSTC 110-2410-H-002-142-MY2) and by the Empower Vocational Education Research Center of National Taiwan University of Science and Technology (NTUST).

Funding

Ministry of Science and Technology (NSTC 110–2410-H-002–142-MY2).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CL designed all experiments; W-KL provided VR technology; W-HL, conducted the experiments; SC, Y-TL, W-HL and CL analyzed the data; CL, Y-TL and W-KL wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caleb Liang.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Informed consent to participate was acquired from all participants. The persons whose body shown in the Figures gave their written informed consent to publish. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. This study was performed in accordance with the regulations of, and was approved by, the Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University (NTU-REC: 202012HM035). The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 64 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Liou, WK., Lin, WH., Lee, YT. et al. The distinction between first-person perspective and third-person perspective in virtual bodily self-consciousness. Virtual Reality 28, 1 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-023-00907-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-023-00907-8

Keywords

Navigation