Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of instructing system limitations on the intervening behavior of drivers in partial driving automation

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognition, Technology & Work Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Proper understanding of automation limitations is vital in order for drivers to deal with unexpected critical situations. The present study focuses on explanation-based knowledge of the limitations given to novice drivers who have no knowledge and experience of using driving automation systems. The knowledge is discussed considering (1) the possibility of the automation failing to issue an alert when the driving automation cannot handle the situation and (2) the manner of describing the limitations from either a functional or scenic point of view. An experiment conducted under 2 × 2 conditions of explanation-based knowledge (n = 24 participants per condition, average age of the participant = 55.9 ± 16.2 years) is implemented in a driving simulator. Data on transition time from automated control to manual control are collected. The results reveal that drivers could intervene more safely if the knowledge is described from a scenic point of view (average ratio of safe intervention = 95%, average reaction time = 2.0 s), as compared to a functional description (88%, 2.3 s). Explicit/scenic knowledge was found to be more beneficial in responding to alerts in such situations involving system limitations as well as in dealing with critical system failures. Further investigation of glance behavior and interviews revealed that novice drivers with explicit/functional knowledge are prone to be over-reliant on the automation’s capability. Therefore, the present study clarified that providing a driver with knowledge about system limitations/failures explicitly while giving is instructive for perceiving and responding to system limitations as well as unexpected hazards due to system failures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alkim TP, Bootsma G, Hoogendoorn SP (2007) Field operational test “the assisted driver”. In: Proceedings of the 2007 IEEE intelligent vehicles symposium, Istanbul, Turkey, pp 1198–1203. https://doi.org/10.1109/ivs.2007.4290281

  • Aven T (2011) On different types of uncertainties in the context of the precautionary principle. Risk Anal 31(10):1515–1525

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beller J, Heesen M (2013) Improving the driver-automation interaction: an approach using automation uncertainty. Hum Factors 55(6):1130–1141. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720813482327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanco M, Atwood J, Vasquez HM, Trimble TE, Fitchett VL, Radlbeck J, Fitch GM, Russell SM, Green GA, Cullinane B, Morgan JF (2015) Human factors evaluation of level 2 and level 3 automated driving concepts. NHTSA (report no. DOT HS 812 182), August: 300. http://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.1874.7361

  • Carsten O, Lai FCH, Barnard Y, Jamson AH, Merat N (2012) Control task substitution in semi-automated driving: does it matter what aspects are automated? Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc 54:747–761. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720812460246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casner SM, Geven RW, Recker MP, Schooler JW (2014) The retention of manual flying skills in automated cockpit. Hum Factors 56(8):1506–1516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casner SM, Hutchins EL, Norman D (2016) The challenges of partial automated driving. Commun ACM 59(5):70–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cha D (2003) Driver workload comparisons among road sections of automated highway systems. In: Proceedings of the society of automotive engineers 2003 world congress (Detroit, MI) (technical paper 2003–01-0119)

  • Damböck D, Weißgerber T, Kienle M, Bengler K (2013) Requirements for cooperative vehicle guidance. In: IEEE (Ed.), Proceedings of the 16th international IEEE annual conference on intelligent transportation systems (ITSC 2013), pp 1656–1661

  • de Winter JCF, Happee R, Martens HM, Stanton NA (2014) Effects of adaptive cruise control and highly automated driving on workload and situation awareness: a review of the empirical evidence. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 27(B):196–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2014.06.016(ISSN 1369-8478)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flemisch F, Schieben A, Schoemig N, Strauss M, Lueke S, Heyden A (2011) Design of human computer interfaces for highly automated vehicles in the EU-project HAVEit. In: Stephanidis C (ed) Universal access in human–computer interaction. Context diversity. UAHCI 2011, vol 6767. Lecture notes in computer science. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Stevens AL (1983) Mental models. LEA, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold C, Dambock D, Lorenz L, Bengler K (2013) “Take over!”—how long does it take to get the driver back into the loop? In: Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society, 57th annual meeting, pp 1938–1942

  • Gold C, Körber M, Hohenberger C, Lechner D (2015) Trust in automation—before and after the experience of take-over scenarios in a highly automated vehicle. Procedia Manuf 3:3025–3032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg J, Tijerina L, Curry R, Artz B, Cathey L, Grant P, Kochhar D, Kozak K, Blommer M (2003) Evaluation of driver distraction using an event detection paradigm. J Transp Res Board 1843(1):1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki T, Itoh M (2013) Human’s overtrust in and overreliance on advanced driver assistance systems: a theoretical framework. Int J Veh Technol. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/951762(Article ID: 951762)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN (1983) Mental models. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kircher K, Larsson AF, Hultgren J (2014) Tactical driving behaviour with different levels of automation. IEEE Trans Intell Transp Syst 15(1):158–167. https://doi.org/10.1109/TITS.2013.2277725

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klunder G, Li M, Minderhoud M (2009) Traffic flow impacts of adaptive cruise control deactivation and (re)activation with cooperative driver behaviour. Transp Res Record J Transp Res Board 2129:145–151. https://doi.org/10.3141/2129-17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Körber M, Cingel A, Zimmermann M, Bengler K (2015a) Vigilance decrement and passive fatigue caused by monotony in automated driving. Procedia Manuf 3:2403–2409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.499

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Körber M, Weiẞgerber T, Kalb L, Blaschke C, Farid M (2015b) Prediction of take-over time in highly automated driving by two psychometric tests. DYNA 82(193):195–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Körber M, Gold C, Lechner D, Bengler K (2016) The influence of age on the take-over of vehicle control in highly automated driving. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 39:19–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Körber M, Baseler E, Bengler K (2017) Introduction matters: manipulating trust in automation and reliance in automated driving. Appl Ergonom 66:1–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuehn M, Vogelpohl T, Vollrath M (2017) Takeover times in highly automated driving (level 3). In: 25th International technical conference on the enhanced safety of vehicles (ESV) national highway traffic safety administration

  • Lank C, Haberstroh M, Wille M (2011) Interaction of human, machine, and environment in automated driving systems. Transp Res Rec J Transp Res Board 2243:138–145. https://doi.org/10.3141/2243-16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsson AF, Kircher K, Hultgren JA (2014) Learning from experience: familiarity with ACC and responding to a cut-in situation in automated driving. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 27:229–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee JD, See KA (2004) Trust in automation: designing for appropriate reliance. Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc 46(1):50–80. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.46.1.50_30392

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma R, Kaber DB (2005) Situation awareness and workload in driving while using adaptive cruise control and a cell phone. Int J Ind Ergon 35:939–953

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magister T, Batista M, Bogdanović L (2006) Measurement of the driver response time in the simulated and real emergency driving situations. PROMET Traffic Transp 18(1):23–32

    Google Scholar 

  • May JF, Balldwin CL (2009) Driver fatigue: the importance of identifying causal factors of fatigue when considering detection and countermeasures technologies. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 12(3):218–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melcher V, Rauh S, Diederichs F, Widlroither H, Bauer W (2015) Take-over requests for automated driving. Procedia Manuf 3:2867–2873

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mioch T, Kroon L, Neerincs MA (2017) Driver readiness model for regulating the transfer from automation to human control. In: Proceedings of the 22nd international conference on intelligent user interfaces, pp 205–213. https://doi.org/10.1145/3025171.3025199

  • National Transportation Safety Board (NTSB) (2017) Collision between a car operating with automated vehicle control systems and a tractor-semitrailer truck. Williston, FL. May 7, 2016. Highway accident report NTSB/HAR-17/02. Washington, DC. https://www.ntsb.gov/investigations/AccidentReports/Reports/HAR1702.pdf. Accessed 28 Dec 2017

  • Naujoks F, Mai C, Neukum A (2015a) The effect of urgency of take-over requests during highly automated driving under distraction conditions. In: Stanton N, Landry S, Di Bucchianico G, Vallicelli A (eds) Advances in human aspects of transportation: part I. AHFE conference, pp 431–438

  • Naujoks F, Purucker C, Neukum A, Wolter S, Steiger R (2015b) Controllability of partially automated driving functions—does it matter whether drivers are allowed to take their hands off the steering wheel? Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 35:185–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naujoks F, Purucker C, Neukum A (2016) Secondary task engagement and vehicle automation—comparing the effects of different automation levels in an on-road experiment. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 38:67–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naujoks F, Forster Y, Wiedemann K, Neukum A (2017a) A human–machine interface for cooperative highly automated driving. Adv Hum Asp Transp. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-41682-3-49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naujoks F, Purucker C, Wiedemann K, Neukum A (2017b) Driving performance at lateral system limits during partially automated driving. Accid Anal Prev 108:147–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.08.027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson J, Strand N, Falcone P, Vinter J (2013) Driver performance in the presence of adaptive cruise control related failures: implications for safety analysis and fault tolerance. In: Proceedings of the 43rd annual IEEE/IFIP conference on dependable systems and networks workshop, Budapest, Hungary, pp 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1109/dsnw.2013.6615531

  • Olson PL (1989) Driver perception response time. SAE technical papers 890731

  • Parasuraman R, Manzey DH (2010) Complacency and bias in human use of automation: an attentional integration. Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc 52(3):381–410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payre W, Cestac J, Dang NT, Vienne F, Delhomme P (2017) Impact of training and in-vehicle task performance on manual control recovery in an automated car. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 46(A):216–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.02.001(ISSN 1369-8478)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng Y, Boyle LN, Ghazizadeh M, Lee JD (2013) Factors affecting glance behavior when interacting with in-vehicle devices: implications from a simulator study. In: Proceedings of the seventh international driving symposium on human factors in driver assessment, training, and vehicle design

  • Politis I, Brewster S, Pollick F (2015) To beep or not to beep?: Comparing abstract versus language-based multimodal driver displays. In: CHI 2015, Seoul, Republic of Korea, 18–23 Apr 2015, pp 3971–3980

  • Rogers M, Zhang Y, Kaber D, Liang Y, Gangakhedkar S (2011) The effects of visual and cognitive distraction on driver situation awareness. In: Harris D (ed) Engineering psychology and cognitive ergonomics. Springer, Berlin, pp 186–195. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21741-8_21

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rudin-Brown CM, Parker HA (2004) Behavioural adaptation to adaptive cruise control (ACC): implications for preventive strategies. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 7:59–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2004.02.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAE International (2016) Taxonomy and definitions for terms related to driving automation systems for on-road motor vehicles. In: J3016, SAE international

  • Saxby DJ, Matthews G, Hitchcock EM, Warm JS, Funke GJ, Gantzer T (2008) Effect of active and passive fatigue on performance using a driving simulator. Proc Hum Fact Ergon Soc Annu Meet 52:1252–1256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seppelt BD, Victor TW (2016) Potential solutions to human factors challenges in road vehicle automation. In: Meyer G, Beiker S (eds) road vehicle automation, vol 3. Springer International Publishing, Basel, pp 131–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-40503-2_11

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Solís-Marcos I, Galvao-Carmona A, Kircher K (2017) Reduced attention allocation during short periods of partially automated driving: an event-related potentials study. Front Hum Neurosci 11:537. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton NA, Young M (2005) Driver behaviour with adaptive cruise control. Ergonomics 48:1294–1313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stockert S, Richardson NT, Lienkamp M (2015) Driving in an increasingly automated world—approaches to improve the driver-automation interaction. Procedia Manuf 3:2889–2896

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strand N, Nilsson J, Karlsson I, Nilsson L (2014) Semi-automated versus highly automated driving in critical situations caused by automation failures. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 27(B):218–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vahidi A, Eskandarian A (2003) Research advances in intelligent collision avoidance and adaptive cruise control. IEEE Trans Intell Transp Syst 4(3):143–153. https://doi.org/10.1109/TITS.2003.821292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Victor TW, Harbluk JL, Engström JA (2005) Sensitivity of eye-movement measures to in-vehicle task difficulty. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 8(2):167–190. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0001-4575(15)00073-1/sbref0235

  • Vollrath M, Schleicher S, Gelau C (2011) The influence of Cruise control and adaptive cruise control on driving behaviour—a driving simulator study. Accid Anal Prev 43:1134–1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2010.12.023

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf F, Zeeb K, Rimini-Döring M, Arnon M, Gauterin F (2013) Effects of human–machine interaction mechanisms on situation awareness in partly automated driving. In: Proceedings of the 16th international IEEE annual conference on intelligent transportation systems (ITSC 2013), The Hague, The Netherlands

  • Yanko MR, Spalek TM (2013) Driving with the wandering mind: the effect that mind-wandering has no driving performance. Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc 56(2):260–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young MS, Brookhuis KA, Wickens CD, Hancock PA (2014) State of science: mental workload in ergonomics. Ergonomics 139:1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2014.956151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeeb K, Buchner A, Schrauf M (2015) What determines the take-over time? An integrated model approach of driver take-over after automated driving. Accid Anal Prev 78:212–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeeb K, Buchner A, Schrauf M (2016) Is take-over time all that matters? The impact of visual-cognitive load on driver take-over quality after conditionally automated driving. Accid Anal Prev 92:230–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2016.04.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present study was conducted as a part of the SIP-adus Project entitled “Strategic Innovation Promotion Program (SIP) Automated Driving Systems/Large-Scale Field Operational Test/HMI”. The authors would also like to thank N. Kashiwazaki, Y. Noguchi, R. Taguchi, Y. Ishitsuka, S. Funoki, K. Toyoda, T. W. Liu, S. Nishina, and R. Umeno for their help in data collection and analyses.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to H. P. Zhou.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Descriptions of events, the HMI, and instructions in the eight scenarios

Scenario

Does system issue an alert in the HMI

Descriptions about system limitations

No.

Event

Functional way

Scenic way

A

Range of visibility is less than 40 m because of heavy fog

Yes

System does not work when traffic lines are not sensed

Visual field becomes worse due to deep fog

A

Pavement marking cannot be detected

Yes

*Not be unexplained in the instruction

A

Heavy snow is falling

Yes

*Not be unexplained in the instruction

B

Lane closed with pylons because of falling objects

Failed

System cannot recognize static objects on the road

Highway lane is closed due to objects on the road

B

Lane closed with pylons because of construction

Failed

*Not be unexplained in the instruction

B

Lane closed without pylons because of a stopped car

Failed

*Not be unexplained in the instruction

C

Car approaches an expressway junction

Yes

System cannot change lanes automatically

Car approaches an expressway junction

C

Lane reduction occurs

Yes

*Not be unexplained in the instruction

Appendix 2: Descriptions of data collection under the four conditions

Procedure

Group

G1: Explicit/functional

G2: Explicit/scenic

G3: Implicit/functional

G4: Implicit/scenic

1. Common instruction

Purpose of data collection

Agreement on informed consent sheet

Manual operation of driving simulator

Exercise on manual operation

Use of automated driving system

Exercise on automated driving

2. Conditioned instruction

Explicit instructions about the possibility that the system will fail to provide an alert

Implicit instructions about the possibility that the system will fail to provide an alert

Functional description of system limitations

Scenic description of system limitations

Functional description of system limitations

Scenic description of system limitations

3. Data collection

12 trials (nine trials in which RtI events occur and three trials in which no RtI events occur)

Note that half of participants were instructed to undergo the 12 trials in Order Aa, whereas the other participants were instructed to undergo the 12 trials in Order Bb

4. Interview

  1. aOrder A: Scene: A (heavy fog) → D (dummy trial) → C′ (lane reduction) → D′ (dummy trial) → B (lane closed) → A′ (lines blurred) → B′ (lane closed) → C (junction) → D″ (dummy trial) → B (lane closed) → A″ (Heavy snow)
  2. bOrder B: Scene: C′ (lane reduction) → B (lane closed) → D (dummy trial) → A (heavy fog) → A′ (lines blurred) → D′ (dummy trial) → A″ (heavy snow)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhou, H.P., Itoh, M. & Kitazaki, S. Effect of instructing system limitations on the intervening behavior of drivers in partial driving automation. Cogn Tech Work 22, 321–334 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-019-00568-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-019-00568-1

Keywords

Navigation