Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Will I start an automated driving system? Report on the emotions, cognition, and intention of drivers who experienced real-world conditional automated driving

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognition, Technology & Work Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The automotive market today has seen the entry of Level-3 conditional automated driving vehicles equipped with an automated driving system that waits for the drivers to start it on the road. Before making a full assessment of the use of automated driving systems, drivers should be made to experience real-world conditional automated driving. A driver may have a mood change when driving a real-world automated vehicle. This emotion points to the mediation of motivation, which affects a driver’s cognition and intention to start an automated driving system on the road. In this study, the emotion of experiencing autonomous driving, cognition, and satisfaction of the driving performance were introduced to construct an intention model to start an automated driving system. Online and off-line questionnaires were adopted, and the emotional response, cognition of automated driving, and intention of 133 drivers who experienced real-world conditional automated driving were determined. Driver experience was assessed in four scenarios as part of emotional tests: during manual driving, during conditional automated driving, during takeover under the influence of the warning system, and during takeover driving. The results of the questionnaire showed a significant positive correlation between emotion and cognition, satisfaction of autonomous driving performance, and the intent to start the automated driving system. Emotions play a mediating role between cognition, satisfaction, and intention to start automated driving. Drivers who experienced conditional automated driving appeared to exhibit a moderately high level of emotional response in terms of joy, interest, and surprise, whereas medium-level negative emotions included fear and anger. Drivers experienced some intensity of emotional changes during conditional automated driving and takeover driving. The emotional changes were uneven but encouraging support was reported. In addition, specific hypotheses relating the driving performance of the automated vehicles (in terms of programmed design of takeover and warning system of takeover) to the emotional dimensions were tested. A cluster analysis of the emotional response measures revealed five different emotional patterns when experiencing the real-world automated vehicle, among which the happy/satisfied group had higher intention to start an automated driving system on the road, followed by the emotional group, whereas the disgust group showed the lowest intention. The cluster analysis was supported by demographic and driving cognitive characteristics (age, education, and self-evaluation of the driving level and driving experience) of the five groups of drivers. Finally, the theoretical and practical significance of this study was expounded. The research results may provide some suggestions and hints for the government and enterprises to promote the development of automated driving.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbink DA, Mulder M, Boer ER (2012) Haptic shared control: smoothly shifting control authority? Cogn Technol Work 14 (1): 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-011-0192-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aeberhard M, Rauch S, Bahram M, Tanzmeister G, Thomas J, Pilat Y, Kaempchen N (2015) Experience, results and lessons learned from automated driving on Germany’s highways. IEEE Intell Transp Syst Mag 7 (1): 42–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alambeigi H, McDonald AD (2021) A Bayesian regression analysis of the effects of alert presence and scenario criticality on automated vehicle takeover performance. Hum Fact 2: 2

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexandre B, Reynaud E, Osiurak F, Navarro J (2018) Acceptance and acceptability criteria: a literature review. Cogn Technol Work 20 (2): 165–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball K, Owsley C, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR (1993) Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Invest Ophthalmol vis Sci 34 (11): 3110–3123

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks VA, Plant KL, Stanton NA (2018) Driver error or designer error: using the perceptual cycle model to explore the circumstances surrounding the fatal tesla crash on 7th May 2016. Saf Sci 108: 278–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer JB, Campbell SW (2012) Texting while driving on automatic: considering the frequency-independent side of habit. Comput Hum Behav 28 (6): 2083–2090

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becheur I, Guizani H, Shaaban K (2019) Belief in fate and self-efficacy in road safety advertising based on guilt: an explanation based on negotiable fate. Austr Market J (AMJ) 27 (4): 233–241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger U, Anaki D (2014) Demographic influences on disgust: evidence from a heterogeneous sample. Personality Individ Differ 64: 67–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borojeni SS, Meschtscherjakov A, Mirnig AG, Boll S, Naujoks F, Politis I, & Alverez I (2017) Control transition workshop: Handover and takeover procedures in highly automated driving. In proceedings of the 9th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications adjunct  (pp. 39–46).

  • Brave S, Nass C (2009) Emotion in human-computer interaction. Hum-Comput Interact Fundam 20094635 (53–68): 4

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown JS, Farber IE (1951) Emotions conceptualized as intervening variables—with suggestions toward a theory of frustration. Psychol Bull 48 (6): 465

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camras L (1980) Emotion: a psychoevolutionary synthesis.

  • Carsten O, Martens MH (2019) How can humans understand their automated cars? HMI principles, problems and solutions. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 3–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Celebi ME, Kingravi HA, Vela PA (2013) A comparative study of efficient initialization methods for the k-means clustering algorithm. Expert Syst Appl 40 (1): 200–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chee PNE, Susilo YO, Wong YD (2021) Longitudinal interactions between experienced users’ service valuations and willingness-to-use a first-/last-mile automated bus service. Travel Behav Soc 22: 252–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chikaraishi M, Khan D, Yasuda B, Fujiwara A (2020) Risk perception and social acceptability of autonomous vehicles: a case study in Hiroshima, Japan. Transp Policy 98: 105–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • China Association of Automobile Manufactures (2020a) Top design of intelligent vehicle has been launched and trillion scale market is waiting for exploring. http: //www.caam.org.cn/chn/38/cate_421/con_5228850.html. Accessed 27 February 2020a (in Chinese)

  • China Association of Automobile Manufactures (2020b) Research and application of automatic driving show new development prospects. http: //www.caam.org.cn/chn/38/cate_421/con_5231345.html. Accessed 5 August 2020b (in Chinese)

  • Darley WK, Lim JS (1992) The effect of consumers’ emotional reactions on behavioral intention: The moderating role of personal relevance and self-monitoring. Psychol Mark 9 (4): 329–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Craen S, Twisk DA, Hagenzieker MP, Elffers H, Brookhuis KA (2011) Do young novice drivers overestimate their driving skills more than experienced drivers? Different methods lead to different conclusions. Accid Anal Prev 43 (5): 1660–1665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon G, Hart PS, Clarke C, O’Donnell NH, Hmielowski J (2020) What drives support for self-driving car technology in the United States? J Risk Res 23 (3): 275–287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du N, Yang XJ, Zhou F (2020a) Psychophysiological responses to takeover requests in conditionally automated driving. Accid Anal Prev 148: 105804

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du N, Zhou F, Pulver EM, Tilbury DM, Robert LP, Pradhan AK, Yang XJ (2020b) Examining the effects of emotional valence and arousal on takeover performance in conditionally automated driving. Transp Res Part C Emerg Technol 112: 78–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dube L, Belanger MC, Trudeau E (1996) The role of emotions in health care satisfaction. Mark Health Serv 16 (2): 45

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot 6 (3–4): 169–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth PC (1994) Some reasons to expect universal antecedents of emotion. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions, 150–154.

  • Fagnant DJ, Kockelman K (2015) Preparing a nation for autonomous vehicles: opportunities, barriers and policy recommendations. Transp Res Part A Policy Pract 77: 167–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer KW, Shaver PR, Carnochan P (1990) How emotions develop and how they organise development. Cogn Emot 4 (2): 81–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner GT, Gould LC (1989) Public perceptions of the risks and benefits of technology 1. Risk Anal 9 (2): 225–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauld CS, Lewis I, White KM (2014) Concealed texting while driving: What are young people’s beliefs about this risky behaviour? Saf Sci 65: 63–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gold C, Körber M, Hohenberger C, Lechner D, Bengler K (2015) Trust in automation–before and after the experience of take-over scenarios in a highly automated vehicle. Proc Manuf 3: 3025–3032

    Google Scholar 

  • Grillon C, Lago T, Stahl S, Beale A, Balderston N, Ernst M (2020) Better cognitive efficiency is associated with increased experimental anxiety. Psychophysiology 57 (8): e13559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo X, Wang Z (2007) Concept, function and meaning of positive emotion. Adv Psychol Sci 15 (05): 810

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones E (2004) Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biol Psychol 67 (1–2): 51–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohenberger C, Spörrle M, Welpe IM (2017) Not fearless, but self-enhanced: The effects of anxiety on the willingness to use autonomous cars depend on individual levels of self-enhancement. Technol Forecast Soc Chang 116: 40–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holden RJ (2011) Cognitive performance-altering effects of electronic medical records: an application of the human factors paradigm for patient safety. Cogn Technol Work 13 (1): 11–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu TY, Xie X, Li J (2013) Negative or positive? The effect of emotion and mood on risky driving. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav 16: 29–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang G, Petersen C, Pitts BJ (2020). The Impact of Mental States on Semi-autonomous Driving Takeover Performance: A Systematic Review. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 1372–1376). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

  • Hutcherson CA, Gross JJ (2011) The moral emotions: a social–functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. J Pers Soc Psychol 100 (4): 719

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki T, Sheridan TB (2019) A critique of the SAE conditional driving automation definition, and analyses of options for improvement. Cogn Technol Work 21 (4): 569–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard CE (1977) Differential emotions theory. Human emotions. Springer, Boston, pp 43–66

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Izard CE (2011) Forms and functions of emotions: matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emot Rev 3 (4): 371–378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1884) What is an emotion? Mind 9: 188–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaleńska-Rodzaj J (2020) Music performance anxiety and pre-performance emotions in the light of psychology of emotion and emotion regulation. Psychol Music. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735620961154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim H, Song M, Doerzaph Z (2020) Real-world use of partially automated driving systems and driver impressions. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 1092–1093). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications.

  • Kohl C, Knigge M, Baader G, Böhm M, Krcmar H (2018) Anticipating acceptance of emerging technologies using twitter: the case of self-driving cars. J Bus Econ 88 (5): 617–642

    Google Scholar 

  • Koo J, Kwac J, Ju W, Steinert M, Leifer L, Nass C (2015) Why did my car just do that? Explaining semi-autonomous driving actions to improve driver understanding, trust, and performance. Int J Interact Des Manuf (IJIDeM) 9 (4): 269–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyriakidis M, Happee R, de Winter JC (2015) Public opinion on automated driving: results of an international questionnaire among 5000 respondents. Transp Res Traffic Psychol Behav 32: 127–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai WT, Chen CF (2011) Behavioral intentions of public transit passengers—The roles of service quality, perceived value, satisfaction and involvement. Transp Policy 18 (2): 318–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lajunen T, Summala H (1995) Driving experience, personality, and skill and safety-motive dimensions in drivers’ self-assessments. Person Individ Differ 19 (3): 307–318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lange CG (1885) The mechanism of the emotions. Classic Psychol 2: 672–684

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (2014) Coming to terms with fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111 (8): 2871–2878

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee YJ (2016) The relationships amongst emotional experience, cognition, and behavioural intention in battlefield tourism. Asia Pac J Tour Res 21 (6): 697–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li S, Blythe P, Guo W, Namdeo A (2019) Investigation of older drivers’ requirements of the human-machine interaction in highly automated vehicles. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav 62: 546–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linnenbrink EA, Pintrich PR (2004) Role of affect in cognitive processing in academic contexts. Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development, 57–87.

  • Liu H, Yang R, Wang L, Liu P (2019a) Evaluating initial public acceptance of highly and fully autonomous vehicles. Int J Hum-Comput Interact 35 (11): 919–931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu P, Xu Z, Zhao X (2019b) Road tests of self-driving vehicles: affective and cognitive pathways in acceptance formation. Transp Res Part A Policy Pract 124: 354–369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu P, Jiang Z, Li T, Wang G, Wang R, Xu Z (2021) User experience and usability when the automated driving system fails: findings from a field experiment. Accid Anal Prev 161: 106383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin D, O’neill M, Hubbard S, Palmer A, (2008) The role of emotion in explaining consumer satisfaction and future behavioural intention. J Serv Market 2: 1

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeely LE, Burton JL, Pennington JR (2020) Emotional and cognitive drivers of customer satisfaction in adventure travel. Tour Rev Int 23 (3–4): 133–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merat N, Jamson AH, Lai FC, Carsten O (2012) Highly automated driving, secondary task performance, and driver state. Hum Fact 54 (5): 762–771

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merat N, Seppelt B, Louw T, Engström J, Lee JD, Johansson E, Keinath A (2019) The “out-of-the-loop” concept in automated driving: proposed definition, measures and implications. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 87–98

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesken J, Hagenzieker MP, Rothengatter T, De Waard D (2007) Frequency, determinants, and consequences of different drivers’ emotions: an on-the-road study using self-reports, (observed) behaviour, and physiology. Transp Res Traffic Psychol Behav 10 (6): 458–475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moon MA, Khalid MJ, Awan HM, Attiq S, Rasool H, Kiran M (2017) Consumer’s perceptions of website’s utilitarian and hedonic attributes and online purchase intentions: a cognitive–affective attitude approach. Span J Market-ESIC 21 (2): 73–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moons I, De Pelsmacker P (2012) Emotions as determinants of electric car usage intention. J Mark Manag 28 (3–4): 195–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moors A, Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR, Frijda NH (2013) Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emot Rev 5 (2): 119–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno AT, Michalski A, Llorca C, Moeckel R (2018) Shared autonomous vehicles effect on vehicle-km traveled and average trip duration. J Adv Transp. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/8969353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nass C, Jonsson IM, Harris H, Reaves B, Endo J, Brave S, Takayama L (2005). Improving automotive safety by pairing driver emotion and car voice emotion. In CHI'05 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems (pp. 1973–1976). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/1056808.1057070

  • Naujoks F, Purucker C, Wiedemann K, Marberger C (2019) Noncritical state transitions during conditionally automated driving on german freeways: effects of non–driving related tasks on takeover time and takeover quality. Hum Factors 61 (4): 596–613

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naujoks F, Mai C, and Neukum A (2014) The effect of urgency of take-over requests during highly automated driving under distraction conditions. Advances in human aspects of transportation, 7 (Part I), 431.

  • Naujoks F, Wiedemann K, Schömig N (2017). The importance of interruption management for usefulness and acceptance of automated driving. In Proceedings of the 9th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications  (pp. 254–263).

  • Nees MA (2019) Safer than the average human driver (who is less safe than me)? Examining a popular safety benchmark for self-driving cars. J Safety Res 69: 61–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Netzer L, Igra L, Anan YB, Tamir M (2015) When bad emotions seem better: experience changes the automatic evaluation of anger. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 6 (7): 797–804

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann PG (2016) Automated car woes–-whoa there! Ubiquity 2016 (July): 1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nezu AM, Nezu CM, Peacock MA, Girdwood CP (2004) Case formulation in cognitive-behavior therapy. In: SN Haynes, EM Heiby (Eds) Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 3. Behavioral assessment (p. 402–426). John Wiley & Sons, Inc

  • Noel L, de Rubens GZ, Sovacool BK, Kester J (2019) Fear and loathing of electric vehicles: the reactionary rhetoric of range anxiety. Energy Res Soc Sci 48: 96–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noordewier MK, Topolinski S, Van Dijk E (2016) The temporal dynamics of surprise. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 10 (3): 136–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver RL (2014) Satisfaction: a behavioral perspective on the consumer: A behavioral perspective on the consumer. Routledge, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver RL, DeSARBO WS (1989) Processing of the satisfaction response in consumption: a suggested framework and research propositions. J Consum Satisf Dissatisf Complain Behav 2 (1): 1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliver R, Westbrook R (1993) Profiles of consumer emotions and satisfaction in ownership and usage. Emotion 6 (13): 12–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Omozik K, Yang Y, Kuntermann I, Hergeth S, and Bengler K (2019). How long does it take to relax? Observation of driver behavior during real-world conditionally automated driving. https://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment/2019/papers/37/, Iowa Research Online, 2019 Driving Assessment Conference, doi: https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1702.

  • Parnell KJ, Stanton NA, Plant KL (2018) Good intentions: drivers’ decisions to engage with technology on the road and in a driving simulator. Cogn Technol Work 20 (4): 597–619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearl TH (2017) Fast & furious: the misregulation of driverless cars. NYU Ann Surv Am l 73: 19

    Google Scholar 

  • Pêcher C, Lemercier C, Cellier JM (2009) The influence of emotions on driving behaviour. Hindawi Publishers, New-York

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters E, Västfjäll D, Gärling T, Slovic P (2006) Affect and decision making: A “hot” topic. J Behav Decis Mak 19 (2): 79–85

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pink S, Osz K, Raats K, Lindgren T, Fors V (2020) Design anthropology for emerging technologies: trust and sharing in autonomous driving futures. Des Stud 69: 100942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush B, Frank RA (1999) Assessing food neophobia: the role of stimulus familiarity. Appetite 32 (2): 261–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raue M, D’Ambrosio LA, Ward C, Lee C, Jacquillat C, Coughlin JF (2019) The influence of feelings while driving regular cars on the perception and acceptance of self-driving cars. Risk Anal 39 (2): 358–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson RD, Meister SR, Vanlaar WG, Hing MM (2017) Automated vehicles and behavioural adaptation in Canada. Transp Res Part A Policy Practice 104: 50–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins S, Novaco RW (1999) Systems conceptualization and treatment of anger. J Clin Psychol 55 (3): 325–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers EM (2010) Diffusion of innovations. Simon and Schuster

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1990) A theory of emotion, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. Cogn Emot 4 (3): 161–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosaldo MZ (1983) The shame of headhunters and the autonomy of self. Ethos 11 (3): 135–151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin DC, Berntsen D (2003) Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Mem Cognit 31 (1): 1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell JA (1979) Affective space is bipolar. J Pers Soc Psychol 37 (3): 345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma P (2020) User psychology of emotional interaction—usability, user experience and technology ethics. Emotions in technology design: From experience to ethics. Springer, Cham, pp 15–26

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sanghavi H, Zhang Y, Jeon M (2020) Effects of anger and display urgency on takeover performance in semi-automated vehicles. In 12th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications (pp. 48–56).

  • Schneemann F, Diederichs F (2019) Action prediction with the Jordan model of human intention: a contribution to cooperative control. Cogn Technol Work 21 (4): 711–721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraagen JM, Elsasser P, Fricke H, Hof M, Ragalmuto F (2020) Trusting the X in XAI: Effects of different types of explanations by a self-driving car on trust, explanation satisfaction and mental models. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting  (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 339–343). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

  • Schwarz C, Gaspar J, Brown T (2019) The effect of reliability on drivers’ trust and behavior in conditional automation. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 41–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist M (2000) The influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Anal 20 (2): 195–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolowska J, Sleboda P (2015) The inverse relation between risks and benefits: the role of affect and expertise. Risk Anal 35 (7): 1252–1267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan JM, Flannagan MJ, Pradhan AK, Bao S (2016) Literature review of behavioral adaptations to advanced driver assistance systems. https://aaafoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/12/BehavioralAdaptationADAS.pdf

  • Taarvig E, Solbakken OA, Grova B, Monsen JT (2016) Exploring affect integration in children with anxiety disorders: a pilot study. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pract 3 (4): 338

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang F, Gao F, Wang Z (2020) Driving capability-based transition strategy for cooperative driving: from manual to automatic. IEEE Access 8: 139013–139022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor J, Deane F, Podd J (2002) Driving-related fear: a review. Clin Psychol Rev 22 (5): 631–645

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Techer F, Ojeda L, Barat D, Marteau JY, Rampillon F, Feron S, Dogan E (2019b) Anger and highly automated driving in urban areas: the role of time pressure. Transport Res Traffic Psychol Behav 64: 353–360

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Techer F, Dogan E, Rampillon F, Ojeda L, Feron S, Barat D, Marteau JY (2019a) Providing contextual information when encountering traffic interruptions during automated driving: a preliminary study. In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications: Adjunct Proceedings (pp. 456–460)

  • Trevarthen C (1977) Cognition and reality: principles and implications of cognitive psychology

  • Turner JH (2009) The sociology of emotions: basic theoretical arguments. Emot Rev 1 (4): 340–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2010) Human-error-based design of barriers and analysis of their uses. Cogn Technol Work 12 (2): 133–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2012) Cooperation and learning to increase the autonomy of ADAS. Cogn Technol Work 14 (1): 61–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2016) Toward a petri net based model to control conflicts of autonomy between cyber-physical & human-systems. IFAC-PapersOnLine 49 (32): 36–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2017) Towards increased systems resilience: New challenges based on dissonance control for human reliability in cyber-physical&human systems. Annu Rev Control 44: 316–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2021a) Heuristic-based method for conflict discovery of shared control between humans and autonomous systems-A driving automation case study. Robot Auton Syst 146: 103867

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F (2021b) Weak signal-oriented investigation of ethical dissonance applied to unsuccessful mobility experiences linked to human–machine interactions. Sci Eng Ethics 27 (1): 1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanderhaegen F, Carsten O (2017) Can dissonance engineering improve risk analysis of human–machine systems? Cogn Technol Work 19 (1): 1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatesh V, Morris MG, Davis GB, Davis FD (2003) User acceptance of information technology: toward a unified view. MIS Q 27 (3): 425–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visschers VH, Siegrist M (2018) Differences in risk perception between hazards and between individuals. Psychological perspectives on risk and risk analysis. Springer, Cham, pp 63–80

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wang X (2014) The role of empathic concern, internalized moral principle, and anticipated guilt in college students’ intentions to send or read text messages while driving. Int J Health Promot Educ 52 (6): 346–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner DM, Bargh JA (1998) Control and automaticity in social life. The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1). McGraw-Hill, Boston.

  • Westbrook RA (1987) Product/consumption-based affective responses and postpurchase processes. J Mark Res 24 (3): 258–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westbrook RA, Oliver RL (1991) The dimensionality of consumption emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction. J Consum Res 18 (1): 84–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wintersberger P, Riener A, Frison AK (2016) Automated driving system, male, or female driver: Who'd you prefer? comparative analysis of passengers' mental conditions, emotional states & qualitative feedback. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications (pp. 51–58).

  • Wulvik AS, Dybvik H, Steinert M (2020) Investigating the relationship between mental state (workload and affect) and physiology in a control room setting (ship bridge simulator). Cogn Technol Work 22 (1): 95–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu G, Wang S, Zhao D (2021) Transition to sustainable transport: understanding the antecedents of consumer’s intention to adopt electric vehicles from the emotional research perspective. Environ Sci Pollut Res 28 (16): 20362–20374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang E, Dorneich M (2015) The effect of time delay on emotion, arousal, and satisfaction in human-robot interaction. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting  (Vol. 59, No. 1, pp. 443–447). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications.

  • Yu Z, Feng Z, Jiang K, Huang Z, Yang Z (2020) Riding personal mobility vehicles on the road: an analysis of the intentions of Chinese users. Cogn Technol Work 22 (4): 801–814

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeeb K, Härtel M, Buchner A, Schrauf M (2017) Why is steering not the same as braking? The impact of non-driving related tasks on lateral and longitudinal driver interventions during conditionally automated driving. Transport Res Traffic Psychol Behav 50: 65–79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang T, Tao D, Qu X, Zhang X, Lin R, Zhang W (2019) The roles of initial trust and perceived risk in public’s acceptance of automated vehicles. Transp Res Part C Emerg Technol 98: 207–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang T, Zeng W, Zhang Y, Tao D, Li G, Qu X (2021) What drives people to use automated vehicles? A meta-analytic review. Accid Anal Prev 159: 106270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zillmann D (1991) Television viewing and physiological arousal. Responding to the screen: Reception and reaction processes. 103–133

  • Zohar D, Tzischinski O, Epstein R (2003) Effects of energy availability on immediate and delayed emotional reactions to work events. J Appl Psychol 88 (6): 1082

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zurawik MA, Snape R, Carson J (2019) ‘Relaxing way to spend a day’or ‘Best way to keep fit and healthy’. Comparison of leisure experiences in rambling and Nordic walking and their contributions to well-being. Int J Sociol Leisure 2 (4): 347–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 51905142, 52172344 and 71971073) and the China Scholarship Council (Grant No. CSC201906695035).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kang Jiang.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yu, Z., Jiang, K., Huang, Z. et al. Will I start an automated driving system? Report on the emotions, cognition, and intention of drivers who experienced real-world conditional automated driving. Cogn Tech Work 24, 641–666 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-022-00706-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-022-00706-2

Keywords

Navigation