Abstract
We are quite often exposed to multiple objects present in the visual scene, thus attentional selection is necessary in order to selectively respond to the relevant information. Objects can be selected on the basis of the location they occupy by orienting attention in space. In this paper, we review the evidence showing that attention can be oriented in space either endogenously, on the basis of central cues, predictive of the relevant location, or exogenously, automatically triggered by the salient properties of visual stimuli (peripheral cues). Several dissociations observed between orienting on the basis of the two types of cues have led to the conclusion that they do not represent just two modes of triggering the orienting of the very same attentional mechanism, but rather they modulate processing differently. We present a theoretical framework according to which endogenous predictive cues facilitate target processing by orienting attention, thus amplifying processing at the attended location. In contrast, apart from attentional orienting, peripherally presented discrepant cues might trigger additional cue-target event-integration and event-segregation processes, which modulate processing in a different way, thus leading to cueing effects that are exclusively triggered by peripheral cues.




Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Anllo-Vento L (1995) Shifting attention in visual space. The effects of peripheral cueing on brain cortical potentials. Int J Neurosci 80:353–370
Arnott SR, Pratt J, Shore DI, Alain C (2001) Attentional set modulates visual areas: an event-related potential study of attentional capture. Cogn Brain Res 12(3):383–395
Bartolomeo P, Chokron S (2002) Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neurosci B 26(2):217–234
Berger A, Henik A (2000) The endogenous modulation of IOR is nasal-temporal asymmetric. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):421–428
Berlucchi G, Chelazzi L, Tassinari G (2000) Volitional covert orienting to a peripheral cue does not suppress cue-induced inhibition of return. J Cogn Neurosci 12(4):648–663
Briand KA (1998) Feature integration and spatial attention: more evidence of a dissociation between endogenous and exogenous orienting. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24(4):1243–1256
Briand KA, Klein RM (1987) Is Posner’s “beam” the same as Treisman’s “glue”?: on the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:228–241
Carrasco M, Penpeci-Talgar C, Eckstein M (2000) Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vision Res 40(10–12):1203–1215
Chica AB, Lupiáñez J (2004) Inhibition of return without return of attention. Psicothema 16(2):248–254
Ciaramitaro VM, Cameron EL, Glimcher PW (2001) Stimulus probability directs spatial attention: an enhancement of sensitivity in humans and monkeys. Vision Res 41(1)57–75
Clark VP, Hillyard SA (1996) Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. J Cogn Neurosci 8(5)387–402
Colby CL, Duhamel JR, Goldberg ME (1996) Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. J Neurophys 76(5):2841–2852
Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(3):201–215
Danziger S, Kingstone A, Ward R (2001) Environmentally defined frames of reference: their time course and sensitivity to spatial cues and attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(2):494–503
Di Russo F, Martinez A, Hillyard SA (2003) Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. Cereb Cortex 13:46–499
Doallo S, Lorenzo-Lopez L, Vizoso C, Holguin SR, Amenedo E, Bara S, et al. (2004) The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: an event-related potentials study. Clin Neurophysiol 115(1):199–210
Dosher BA, Lu ZL (2000) Noise exclusion in spatial attention. Psychol Sci 11(2):139–146
Downing CJ (1988) Expectancy and visual-spatial attention: effects on perceptual quality. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14(2):188–202
Egly R, Driver J, Rafal RD (1994) Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion patients. J Exp Psychol Gen 123(2):161–177
Eimer M (1993) Spatial cueing, sensory gating and selective response preparation—an ERP study on visuospatial orienting. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 88(5):408–420
Eimer M (2000) The time course of spatial orienting elicited by central and peripheral cues: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biol Psychol 53:253–258
Folk CL, Remington R (1998) Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24(3):847–858
Folk CL, Remington R (1999) Can new objects override attentional control settings? Percept Psychophys 61(4):727–739
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC (1992) Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18(4):1030–1044
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wright JH (1994) The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20(2):317–329
Fu SM, Fan SL, Chen L, Zhuo Y (2001) The attentional effects of peripheral cueing as revealed by two event-related potential studies. Clin Neurophysiol 112(1):172–185
Funes MJ (2004) The role of attentional orienting on the spatial processing of visual stimuli. Doctoral Thesis, Universidad de Granada
Funes MJ, Lupianez J (2003) Posner’s theory of attention: a task to measure the attentional functions of orienting, alerting and cognitive control and the interactions between them. Psicothema 15(2):260–266
Goldsmith M, Yeari M (2003) Modulation of object-based attention by spatial focus under endogenous and exogenous orienting. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(5):897–918
Goschke T (2003) Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In: Maasen S, Prinz W, Roth G (eds) Voluntary action. An issue at the interface of nature and culture. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 49–85
Handy TC, Jha AP, Mangun GR (1999) Promoting novelty in vision: inhibition of return modulates perceptual-level processing. Psychol Sci 10(2):157–161
Harter MR, Aine C, Schroeder C (1982) Hemispheric-differences in the neural processing of stimulus location and type—effects of selective attention on visual evoked-potentials. Neuropsychologia 20(4):421–438
Hawkins HL, Hillyard SA, Luck SJ, Mouloua M, Downing CJ, Woodward DP (1990) Visual attention modulates signal detectability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16(4):802–811
Heinze HJ, Mangun GR, Burchert W, Hinrichs H, Scholz M, Munte TF, et al. (1994) Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature 372(6506):543–546
Hillyard SA, Mangun GR (1987) Sensory gating as a physiological mechanism for visual selective attention. In: Johnson R, Rohrbaugh JW, Parasuraman R (eds) Current trends in event-related potential research. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 61–67
Hoffmann J (1993) Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion von Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Hogrefe, Göttingen
Hommel B (1993a) The relationship between stimulus-processing and response selection in the Simon task—evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychol Res 55(4):280–290
Hommel B (1993b) The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychol Res 55(3):208–222
Hommel B (1998) Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Vis Cogn 5(1–2):183–216
Hommel B, Colzato L (2004) Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Vis Cogn 11(4):483–521
Hopfinger JB, Jha AP, Hopf JM, Girelli M, Mangun GR (2000) Electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies of voluntary and reflexive attention. In: Driver J, Monsell S (eds) Attention and performance XVIII: the control over cognitive processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 125–153
Hopfinger JB, Mangun GR (1998) Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychol Sci 9(6):441–447
Hopfinger JB, Mangun GR (2001) Tracking the influence of reflexive attention on sensory and cognitive processing. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 1(1):56–65
Hunt AR, Kingstone A (2003) Inhibition of return: dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(5):1068–1074
Ivanoff J, Klein RM, Lupiáñez J (2002) Inhibition of return interacts with the Simon effect: an omnibus analysis and its implications. Percept Psychophys 64(2):318–327
Jonides J (1976) Voluntary vs reflexive control of minds eyes movement. Bull Psychon Soc 8(4):243–244
Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In: Long J, Baddeley A (eds) Attention and performance XI. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 187–283
Jonides J, Yantis S (1988) Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Percept Psychophys 43(4):346–354
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ (1992) The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cog Psychol 24(2):175–219
Kane MJ, May CP, Hasher L, Rahhal T, Stoltzfus ER (1997) Dual mechanisms of negative priming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23(3):632–650
Kingstone A, Pratt J (1999) Inhibition of return is composed of attentional and oculomotor processes. Percept Psychophys 61(6):1046–1054
Klein RM (1988) Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature 334:430–431
Klein RM (1994) Perceptual-motor expectancies interact with covert visual orienting under conditions of endogenous but not exogenous control. Can J Exp Psychol 48(2):167–181
Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci 4(4):138–147
Klein RM (2004) On the control of visual orienting. In: Posner MI (ed) Cognitive neuroscience of attention. Guilford, New York (in press)
Klein RM, MacInnes WJ (1999) Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychol Sci 10(4):346–352
Klein RM, Shore DI (2000) Relations among modes of visual orienting. In: Monsell S, Driver J (eds) Attention and performance XVIII: control of cognitive processes. MIT, Cambridge, pp 195–208
Klein RM, Taylor TL (1994) Categories of cognitive inhibition with reference to attention. In: Dale Dagenbach THC (ed) Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. Academic, San Diego, pp 113–150
Kornblum S (1992) Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus compatibility. In: Stelmach G, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior II. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 743–777
Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A (1990) Dimensional overlap—cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev 97(2):253–270
Kornblum S, Lee JW (1995) Stimulus–response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(4):855–875
Làdavas E, Carletti M, Gori G (1994) Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect: horizontal and vertical dimensions. Neuropsychologia 32:1195–1208
Losier BJ, Klein RM (2001) A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neuroscience B 25(1):1–13
Lowe DG (1985) Further investigations of inhibitory mechanisms in attention. Mem Cogn 13(1):74–80
Lu ZL, Dosher BA (1998) External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Res 38(9):1183–1198
Lu ZL, Dosher BA (2000) Spatial attention: different mechanisms for central and peripheral temporal precues? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26(5):1534–1548
Lupiáñez J, Decaix C, Sieroff E, Chokron S, Milliken B, Bartolomeo P (2004) Independent effects of endogenous and exogenous spatial cueing: inhibition of return at endogenously attended target locations. Exp Brain Res 159(4):447–457
Lupiáñez J, Funes MJ (2004) Peripheral spatial cues modulate spatial congruency effects: analyzing the “locus” of the cueing modulation. Eur J Cogn P (in press)
Lupiáñez J, Milan EG, Tornay FJ, Madrid E, Tudela P (1997) Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes, it does, but later. Percept Psychophys 59(8):1241–1254
Lupiáñez J, Milliken B (1999) Inhibition of return and the attentional set for integrating versus differentiating information. J Gen Psychol 126(4):392–418
Lupiáñez J, Milliken B, Solano C, Weaver B, Tipper SP (2001) On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. Q J Exp Psychol A 54(3):753–773
Lupiáñez J, Ruz H, Funes MJ, Hilliken B (2004) Psychological Research (submitted)
Lupiáñez J, Tudela P, Rueda MR (1999) Control inhibitorio en la orientacion atencional: una revision sobre la inhibicion de retorno./Inhibitory control in attentional orientation: a review about the inhibition of return. Cognitiva 11(1):23–44
Macquistan AD (1997) Object-based allocation of visual attention in response to exogenous, but not endogenous, spatial precues. Psychon Bull Rev 4(4):512–515
Mangun GR (1995) Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention in humans. Psychophysiology 32(1):4–18
Mangun GR, Hillyard SA (1987) The spatial allocation of visual-attention as indexed by event-related brain potentials. Hum Factors 29(2):195–211
Mangun GR, Hillyard SA (1991) Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual spatial priming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17(4):1057–1074
Mangun GR, Hillyard SA, Luck SJ (1993) Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention. Atten Perform 14:219–243
Mangun GR, Hinrichs H, Scholz M, Mueller-Gaertner HW, Herzog H, Krause BJ et al. (2001) Integrating electrophysiology and neuroimaging of spatial selective attention to simple isolated visual stimuli. Vision Res 41(10–11):1423–1435
Mangun GR, Hopfinger JB, Kussmaul CL, Fletcher EM, Heinze HJ (1997) Covariations in ERP and PET measures of spatial selective attention in human extrastriate visual cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 5(4):273–279
Marczinski CA, Milliken B, Nelson S (2003) Aging and repetition effects: separate specific and nonspecific influences. Psychol Aging 18(4):780–790
Martinez A, Anllo-Vento L, Sereno MI, Frank LR, Buxton RB, Dubowitz DJ, et al. (1999) Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nat Neurosci 2(4):364–369
Maruff P, Yucel M, Danckert J, Stuart G, Currie J (1999) Facilitation and inhibition arising from the exogenous orienting of covert attention depends on the temporal properties of spatial cues and targets. Neuropsychologia 37(6):731–744
Maunsell JHR, McAdams CJ (2000) Effects of attention on neuronal response properties in visual cerebral cortex. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The new cognitive neurosciences. MIT, Cambridge, pp 290–305
Milliken B (2002) Commentary on Ruz and Lupiáñez’s “a review of attention capture: on its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control”. Psicológica 23:355–356
Milliken B, Joordens S, Merikle PM, Seiffert AE (1998) Selective attention: a reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychol Rev 105(2):203–229
Milliken B, Tipper SP, Houghton G, Lupiáñez J (2000) Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Percept Psychophys 62(6):1280–1296
Mort DJ, Perry RJ, Mannan SK, Hodgson TL, Anderson E, Quest R, et al. (2003) Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. Neuroimage 18(2):231–246
Müller HJ, Rabbitt PM (1989) Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15(2):315–330
Muller HJ, von Muhlenen A (2000) Probing distracter inhibition in visual search: inhibition of return. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26(5):1591–1605
Oonk HM, Abrams RA (1998) New perceptual objects that capture attention produce inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 5(3):510–515
Peelen MV, Heslenfeld DJ, Theeuwes J (2004) Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. Neuroimage 22(2):822–830
Pollmann S, Weidner R, Humphreys GW, Olivers CNL, Muller K, Lohmann G, et al. (2003) Separating distractor rejection and target detection in posterior parietal cortex—an event-related fMRI study of visual marking. Neuroimage 18(2):310–323
Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25
Posner MI, Cohen Y (1984) Components of visual orienting. In: Bouma H, Bouwhuis D (eds) Attention and performance X. Lawrence Erlbaum, London, pp 531–556
Posner MI, Nissen M, Odgen W (1978) Attended and unattended processing modes: the role of set for spatial location. In: Pick HL, Saltzman E (eds) Modes of perceiving and processing information. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 128–181
Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS, Vaughan J (1985) Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn Neuropsychol 2:211–228
Posner MI, Snyder CRR, Davidson BJ (1980) Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:160–174
Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD (1984) Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 4:1863–1874
Prime DJ, Ward LM (2004) Inhibition of return from stimulus to response. Psychol Sci 15(4):272–276
Proctor RW, Lu CH, Vanzandt T (1992) Enhancement of the Simon effect by response precuing. Acta Psychol 81(1):53–74
Rafal R (1998) Neglect. In: Parasuraman R (ed) The attentive brain. MIT, Cambridge, pp 489–525
Rafal R, Henik A (1994) The neurology of inhibition: integrating controlled and automatic processes. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH (eds) Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language. Academic, San Diego, pp 1–51
Reynolds JH, Desimone R (2001) Neural mechanisms of attentional selection. In: Braun J, Koch C, Davis JL (eds) Visual attention and cortical circuits. MIT, Cambridge, pp 121–135
Rosen AC, Rao SM, Caffarra P, Scaglioni A, Bobholz JA, Woodley SJ et al (1999) Neural basis of endogenous and exogenous spatial orienting. A functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 11(2):135–152
Rubichi S, Nicoletti R, Iani C, Umilta C (1997) The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23(5):1353–1364
Ruz M, Lupiáñez J (2002) A review of attentional capture: on its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicológica 23:283–309
Shalev L, Algom D (2000) Stroop and Garner effects in and out of Posner’s beam: reconciling two conceptions of selective attention [in process citation]. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26(3):997–1017
Stoffer TH (1991) Attentional focusing and spatial stimulus–response compatibility. Psychol Res 53(2):127–135
Stoffer TH, Umilta C (1997) Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S–R compatibility and the Simon effect. In: Hommel B, Prinz W (eds) Theoretical issues in S–R compatibility. Amsterdam, North-Holland, pp 181–128
Stoffer TH, Yakin AR (1994) The functional-role of attention for spatial coding in the Simon effect. Psychol Res 56(3):151–162
Takeda Y, Yagi A (2000) Inhibitory tagging in visual search can be found if search stimuli remain visible. Percept Psychophys 62(5):927–934
Taylor TL, Donnelly MP (2002) Inhibition of return for target discriminations: the effect of repeating discriminated and irrelevant stimulus dimensions. Percept Psychophys 64(2):447–457
Theeuwes J (1991) Exogenous and endogenous control of attention—the effect of visual onsets and offsets. Percept Psychophys 49(1):83–90
Theeuwes J (1992) Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept Psychophys 51(6):599–606
Theeuwes J (1994) Endogenous and exogenous control of visual selection. Perception 23(4):429–440
Thompson KG, Bichot NP, Schall JD (1997) Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. J Neurophys 77(2):1046–1050
Tipper SP, Driver J, Weaver B (1991) Object-centred inhibition of return of visual attention. Q J Exp Psychol A 43A:289–298
Tipper SP, Weaver B, Watson FL (1996) Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations: commentary on Pratt and Abrams (1995) J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22(5):1289–1293
Treisman A, Gelade G (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12(1):97–136
Verfaellie M, Bowers D, Heilman KM (1988) Attentional factors in the occurrence of stimulus–response compatibility effects. Neuropsychologia 26(3):435–444
Vivas AB, Fuentes LJ (2001) Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 8(2):315–323
Yamaguchi S, Tsuchiya H, Kobayashi S (1994) Electroencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. Brain 117:553–562
Yantis S (1998) Control of visual attention. In: Pashler H (ed) Attention. Psychology, London, pp 223–256
Yantis S (2000) Goal directed and stimulus driven determinants of attentional control. In: Monsell S, Driver J (eds) Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance XVIII. MIT, Cambridge, pp 73–103
Yantis S, Egeth HE (1999) On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25(3):661–676
Yantis S, Hillstrom AP (1994) Stimulus-driven attentional capture—evidence from equiluminant visual objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20(1):95–107
Yantis S, Jonides J (1984) Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention—evidence from visual-search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10(5):601–621
Yantis S, Jonides J (1996) Attentional capture by abrupt onsets: new perceptual objects or visual masking? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22(6):1505–1513
Zimba LD, Brito CF (1995) Attention precuing and Simon effects—a test of the attention coding account of the Simon effect. Psychol Res 58(2):102–118
Acknowledgements
This research was financially supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología with a research project grant (BSO2002-04308-C02-02) to the second author. We would like to thank the two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript. Please direct correspondence to María Jesús Funes (e-mail: mjfunes@ugr.es) or to Juan Lupiáñez (e-mail: jlupiane@ugr.es), both at the Departamento de Psicología Experimental y Fisiología del Comportamiento, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Campus Universitario de Cartuja s/n, 18071-Granada, Spain. http://www.ugr.es/neurocog/
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Funes, M.J., Lupiáñez, J. & Milliken, B. The role of spatial attention and other processes on the magnitude and time course of cueing effects. Cogn Process 6, 98–116 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0038-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0038-7