Abstract
Several experimental studies have shown that there exists an association between emotion words and the vertical spatial axis. However, the specific conditions under which this conceptual–physical interaction emerges are still unknown, and no study has been devised to test whether longer linguistic units than words can lead to a mapping of emotions on vertical space. In Experiment 1, Spanish and Colombian participants performed a representative verbal emotional contexts production task (RVEC task) requiring participants to produce RVEC for the emotions of joy, sadness, surprise, anger, fear, and disgust. The results showed gender and cultural differences regarding the average number of RVEC produced. The most representative contexts of joy and sadness obtained in Experiment 1 were used in Experiment 2 in a novel spatial–emotional congruency verification task (SECV task). After reading a sentence, the participants had to judge whether a probe word, displayed in either a high or low position on the screen, was congruent or incongruent with the previous sentence. The question was whether the emotion induced by the sentence could modulate the responses to the probes as a function of their position in a vertical axis by means of a metaphorical conceptual–spatial association. Overall, the results indicate that a mapping of emotions on vertical space can occur for linguistic units larger than words, but only when the task demands an explicit affective evaluation of the target.
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-014-0620-6/MediaObjects/10339_2014_620_Fig1_HTML.gif)
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-014-0620-6/MediaObjects/10339_2014_620_Fig2_HTML.gif)
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-014-0620-6/MediaObjects/10339_2014_620_Fig3_HTML.gif)
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-014-0620-6/MediaObjects/10339_2014_620_Fig4_HTML.gif)
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
In the study reported herein, the focus is only on the number of verbal contexts since potential qualitative differences require different analyses that exceed the limits of this paper.
Figure 2A might suggest that while there was a significant difference between males and women in the Colombian sample, this was not the case in the Spanish sample. Welch pairwise tests showed that those differences were significant in both cases [t(61.09) = 4.96, p < .001 (Colombiamale vs. females) and t(66.75) = 2.84, p = .005 (Spain male vs. females)], thus confirming our second hypothesis, i.e. women produce more RVEC than men. Additionally, a Welch pairwise test showed that there was no significant difference between Colombian and Spanish males t(48.08) = 1.44, p = .15) in Fig. 2a.
We thank one of reviewers for suggesting this analysis.
References
Adolphs R (2001) The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11(2):231–239
Altarriba J, Bauer LM (2004) The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: a comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. Am J Psychol 117(3):389–410
Ansorge U, Bohner G (2013) Investigating the association between valence and elevation with an implicit association task that requires upward and downward responding. Univ Psychol 12(5):1453–1471
Ansorge U, Khalid S, König P (2013) Space-valence priming with subliminal and supraliminal words. Front Psychol 4:81. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00081
Ballesteros S, Millar S, Reales J (1998) Symmetry in haptic and visual shape perception. Percept Psychophys 60(3):389–404
Basabe N, Paez D, Valencia J, Rimé B, Pennebaker J, Diener E, González JL (2000) Sociocultural factors predicting subjective experience of emotion. Psicothema 12:55–69
Bhullar N (2013) Rating of intensity of emotions across auditory, visual, and auditory-visual sensory domains. Curr Psychol 32(2):186–196
Brebner J (2003) Gender and emotions. Pers Individ Differ 34(3):387–394
Brody LR, Hall JA (2008) Gender and emotion in context. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Feldman-Barrett L (eds) Handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 385–408
Carou IF, Redondo J, Piñeiro A, Padrón I, Fernández-Rey J, Alcaraz M (2011) Attentional processing and recall of emotional words. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología 43(3):401–418
de la Vega I, Filippis M, Lachmair M, Dudschig C, Kaup B (2012) Emotional valence and physical space: limits of interaction. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(2):375–385
Elosúa MR, González A (1989) Una tipificación de contextos verbales afectivos. Rev de Psicología Universitas Tarraconensis 11(2):41–53
Fernández I, Carrera P, Sánchez F, Paez D, Candia L (2000) Differences between cultures in emotional verbal and non-verbal reactions. Psicothema 12(1):83–92
Fernández I, Carrera P, Paez D, Sánchez F (2008) Interdependent self-construal, competitive attitudes, culture and emotional reactions on sadness. Psychologia 51:214–234
Fischer AH, Evers C (2010) Anger in the context of gender. In: Potegal M, Stemmler G, Spielberger CD (eds) Handbook of anger. Springer, New York, pp 349–360
Gallese V, Keysersa C, Rizzolatti G (2004) An unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8(9):396–403
Glenberg AM, Sato M, Cattaneo L (2008a) Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Curr Biol 18(7):R290–R291
Glenberg AM, Sato M, Cattaneo L, Riggio L, Palumbo D, Buccino G (2008b) Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Q J Exp Psychol 61(6):905–919
Goldman A, de Vignemont F (2009) Is social cognition embodied? Trends Cogn Sci 13(4):154–159
Gozli DG, Chow A, Chasteen AL, Pratt J (2013) Valence and vertical space: saccade trajectory deviations reveal metaphorical spatial activation. Vis Cogn 21(5):628–646
Inglehart R, Foa R, Peterson P, Welzel C (2008) Development, freedom, and rising happiness. Perspect Psychol Sci 3(4):264–285
Jacob P, Jeannerod M (2005) The motor theory of social cognition: a critique. Trends Cogn Sci 9(1):21–25
Ko SG, Lee TH, Yoon HY, Kwon JH, Mather M (2011) How does context affect assessments of facial emotion? The role of culture and age. Psychol Aging 26(1):48–59
Kousta ST, Vigliocco G, Vinson DP, Andrews M, Del Campo E (2011) The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. J Exp Psychol Gen 140(1):14–34
Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Marmolejo-Ramos F, Dunn J (2013) On the activation of sensorimotor systems during the processing of emotionally-laden stimuli. Univ Psychol 12(5):1511–1542
Marmolejo-Ramos F, Elosúa MR, Yamada Y, Hamm NF, Noguchi K (2013) Appraisal of space words and allocation of emotion words in bodily space. PLoS ONE 8(12):e81688. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0081688
Meier BP, Robinson MD (2004) Why the sunny side is up: associations between affect and vertical position. Psychol Sci 15(4):243–247
Meier BP, Robinson MD (2006) Does “feeling down” mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. J Res Pers 40(4):451–461
Meier BP, Moller AC, Chen JJ, Riemer-Peltz M (2011) Spatial metaphor and real estate: north-south location biases housing preference. Soc Psychol Pers Sci 2(5):547–553
Meier BP, Schnall S, Schwarz N, Bargh JA (2012) Embodiment in social psychology. Top Cogn Sci 4(4):705–716
Meteyard L, Rodríguez S, Bahrami B, Vigliocco G (2012) Coming of age: a review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex 48:788–804
Milovchevich D, Howells K, Drew N, Day A (2001) Sex and gender role differences in anger: an Australian community study. Pers Individ Differ 31(2):117–127
Moseley R, Carota F, Hauk O, Mohr B, Pulvermüller F (2012) A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cereb Cortex 22(7):1634–1647
Neel R, Becker DV, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT (2012) Who expressed what emotion? Men grab anger, women grab happiness. J Exp Soc Psychol 48(2):583–586
Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P, Krauth-Gruber S, Ric F (2005) Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 9(3):184–211
Niedenthal PM, Winkielman P, Mondillon L, Vermeulen N (2009) Embodiment of emotion concepts. J Pers Soc Psychol 96(6):1120–1136
Philippot P, Rimé B (1997) The perception of bodily sensations during emotion: a cross-cultural perspective. Pol J Bull 28(2):175–188
Raposo A, Moss H, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK (2009) Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words, and action sentences. Neuropsychologia 47(2):388–396
Rimé B (2011) La compartición social de las emociones. Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer. (Original Le partage social des émotions. Bruxelles: Mardaga, 2005)
Rosch E, Mervis CB, Gray WD, Johnson DM, Boyes-Braem P (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cogn Psychol 8(3):382–439
Russell JA (1991) Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychol Bull 110:426–450
Russell JA, Barrett LF (1999) Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J Pers Soc Psychol 76:805–819
Sakreida K, Scorolli C, Menz MM, Heim S, Borghi AM, Binkofski F (2013) Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 7:125. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00125
Santiago J, Ouellet M, Román A, Valenzuela J (2012) Attentional factors in conceptual congruency. Cogn Sci 36(6):1051–1077
Saxbe DE, Yang X-F, Borofsky LA, Immordino-Yang MH (2012) The embodiment of emotion: language use during the feeling of social emotions predicts cortical somatosensory activity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. doi:10.1093/scan/nss075
Shaver P, Schwartz J, Kirson D, O’Connor C (1987) Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 52(6):1061–1086
Shields SA (2000) Thinking about gender, thinking about theory: gender and emotional experience. In: Fischer AH (ed) Gender and emotion: social psychological perspectives. Univesity Press & Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’ Homme, Cambridge, pp 3–23
Velasco C, Salgado-Montejo A, Marmolejo-Ramos F, Spence C (2014) Predictive packaging design: tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Qual Prefer 34:88–95
Whelan R (2008) Effective analysis on reaction time tasks. Psychol Rec 58:475–782
Wiemer-Hastings K, Xu X (2005) Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cogn Sci 29(5):719–736
Zanolie K, van Dantzig S, Boot I, Wijnen J, Schubert TW, Giessner SR, Pecher D (2012) Mighty metaphors: behavioural and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain Cogn 78(1):50–58
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Juan Botella (Autónoma University, Madrid) and Antonio Rodán (CEU University, Madrid) for providing facilities to take data for Experiment 1, Carlos Elias Cifuentes Villalobos (Los Libertadores University) for assisting with part of the analyses for Experiment 1, and Dr. Robyn Groves for assisting with proofreading this manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This article is part of the Special Section on “Embodied Social Cognition”, guest-edited by Fernando Marmolejo Ramos and Amedeo Dangiulli.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Marmolejo-Ramos, F., Montoro, P.R., Elosúa, M.R. et al. The activation of representative emotional verbal contexts interacts with vertical spatial axis. Cogn Process 15, 253–267 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0620-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0620-6