Skip to main content
Log in

I believe I’m good at orienting myself… But is that true?

  • Short Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study aimed to analyse beliefs that men and women have with respect to their sense of direction (SOD) and whether they correlate with spatial environmental task performance. Eighty-four students filled in the short version of the Familiarity and Spatial Cognitive Style Scale to evaluate beliefs on their SOD, knowledge of the city (TK), spatial ability (SA) and wayfinding (WA) and performed three spatial environmental tasks. Results showed that gender did not predict the performance on the spatial environmental tasks, whereas it can be predicted by participants’ beliefs related to their SOD and TK. The findings point out the need to identify specific training aimed at improving women’s metacognitive skills in order to delete or reduce gender differences in SA.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

References

  • Bandura A (1989) Social cognitive theory. In: Vasta R (ed) Annals of child development. Six theories of child development, vol 6. JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, pp 1–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry JM, West RL (1993) Cognitive self-efficacy in relation to personal mastery and goal setting across the life span. Int J Behav Dev 16:351–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bipp T, Steinmayr R, Spinath B (2012) A functional look at goal orientations: their role for self-estimates of intelligence and performance. Learn Individ Differ 22:180–189. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.01.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosco A, Longoni AM, Vecchi T (2004) Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Appl Cogn Psych 18:519–532. doi:10.1002/acp.1000

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant KJ (1991) Geographical/spatial orientation ability within real word and simulated large scale environments. Multivar Behav Res 26(1):109–136. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2601_6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell SM, Collaer ML (2009) Stereotype threat and gender differences in performance on a novel visuospatial task. Psychol Women Quart 33(4):437–444. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01521.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carretti B, Borella E, Zavagnin M, De Beni R (2011) Impact of metacognition and motivation on the efficacy of strategic memory training in older adults: analysis of specific, transfer and maintenance effects. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 28(4):331–347. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2010.11.004

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavallini E, Pagnin A, Vecchi T (2003) Aging and everyday memory: the beneficial effect of memory training. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 37:241–257. doi:10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00063-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh JC, Green EE (1990) 7 I believe, therefore I can: self-efficacy beliefs in memory aging. Adv Phsychol 72:189–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J, Choen P (1975) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioural sciences. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Coluccia E, Louse G (2004) Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. J Environ Psychol 24:329–340. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Lisi R, Wolford JL (2002) Improving children’s mental rotation accuracy with computer game playing. J Genet Psychol 163:272–282

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning D, Johnson K, Ehrlinger J, Kruger J (2003) Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 12:83–86. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck CS, Leggett EL (1988) A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol Rev 95:256–273. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng J, Spence I, Pratt J (2007) Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychol Sci 18:850–855

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field A (2005) Discovering statistic using SPSS. SAGE Publication, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley JE, Cohen AJ (1984) Mental mapping of a megastructure. Can J Psychol 38(3):440–453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goff PA, Steele CM, Davies PG (2008) The space between us. J Personal Soc Psychol 94:91–107. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty M, Richardson AE, Montello DR, Lovelace K, Subbiah I (2002) Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. Intelligence 30:425–447

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Dixon RA (1987) Age differences in metamemory: resolving the inconsistencies. Can J Psychol 41:193–208

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein SB, Loftus J (1993) The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In: Skrull T, Wyer R (eds) Advances in social cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 1–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig AM, Eagly AH (2005) Stereotype threat in men on a test of social sensitivity. Sex Roles 52:489–496. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-3714-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman ME, Bandura M, Weaver SL, Elliott E (1995) Assessing memory control beliefs: the memory controllability inventory. Aging Neuropsychol C 2:67–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton CA (2010) Gender, spatial abilities, and wayfinding. In: Chrisler J, McCreary D (eds) Handbook of gender research in psychology. Springer, New York, pp 317–341

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton CA, Charleston SI, Zieles AS (1996) Individual-and gender-related differences in indoor wayfinding. Environ Behav 28:204–219. doi:10.1177/0013916596282003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens A, Johns M, Greenberg J, Schimel J (2006) Combating stereotype threat: the effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. J Exp Soc Psychol 42:236–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGlone MS, Aronson J (2006) Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. J Appl Dev Psychol 27:486–493. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2006.06.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGlone MS, Aronson J, Kobrynowicz D (2006) Stereotype threat and the gender gap in political knowledge. Psychol Women Quart 30:392–398. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00314.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moè A (2012) Gender difference does not mean genetic difference: externalizing improves performance in mental rotation. Learn Individ Differ 22:20–24. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moè A, Pazzaglia F (2010) Beyond genetics in mental rotation test performance. The power of effort attribution. Learn Individ Differ 20:464–468. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.03.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moè A, Meneghetti C, Cadinu M (2009) Women and mental rotation: incremental theory and spatial strategy use enhance performance. Personal Individ Differ 46:187–191. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.030

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nardi D, Newcombe NS, Shipley TF (2013) Reorienting with terrain slope and landmarks. Mem Cognit 41:214–228. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0254-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuburger S, Jansen P, Heil M, Quaiser-Pohl C (2012) A threat in the classroom: gender stereotype activation and mental-rotation performance in elementary-school children. Z Psychol 220(2):61–69. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000097

    Google Scholar 

  • Nori R, Piccardi L (2011) Familiarity and spatial cognitive style: How important are they for spatial representation? In: Thomas JB (ed) Spatial memory: visuospatial processes, cognitive performance and developmental effects. Nova Science, New York, pp 123–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH (1994) Psychometric theory, 3rd edn. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Laughlin EM, Brubaker BS (1998) Use of landmarks in cognitive mapping: gender differences in self report versus performance. Personal Indivd Differ 24(595–60):1. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00237-7

    Google Scholar 

  • Piccardi L, Iaria G, Ricci M, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C (2008) Walking in the Corsi test: which type of memory do you need? Neurosci Lett 432(2):127–131. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.044

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piccardi L, Risetti M, Nori R (2011a) Familiarity and environmental representations of a city: a self-report study. Psychol Rep 109(1):309–326. doi:10.2466/01.13.17.PR0.109.4.309-326

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piccardi L, Risetti M, Nori R, Tanzilli A, Bernardi L, Guariglia C (2011b) Perspective changing in primary and secondary learning: a gender difference study. Learn Individ Differ 21(1):114–118. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piccardi L, Palermo L, Nori R, Giusberti F, Guariglia C (2012) landmark recognition and mental route navigation disorders in patients with imagery neglect and perceptual neglect. In: Spiteri Y, Galea M (eds) Psychology of neglect. Nova Science, New York, pp 81–103

    Google Scholar 

  • Picucci L, Caffò AO, Bosco A (2011) Besides navigation accuracy: gender differences in strategy selection and level of spatial confidence. J Environ Psychol 31(4):430–438. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.01.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prestopnik JL, Roskos-Ewoldsen B (2000) The relations among wayfinding strategy use, sense of direction, sex, familiarity, and wayfinding ability. J Environ Psychol 20:177–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez G, Gunderson EA, Levine SC, Beilock SL (2013) Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. J Cognit Dev 14(2):187–202. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.664593

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shih M, Pittinsky TL, Ambady N (1999) Stereotype susceptibility: identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychol Sci 10:80–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel AW, White SH (1975) The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. In: Reese HW (ed) Advances in child development and behavior, vol 10. Academic Press, New York, pp 9–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer SJ, Steele CM, Quinn DM (1999) Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol 35:4–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verde P, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Trivelloni P, Guariglia C, Tomao E (2013) Gender effects on mental rotation in pilots vs. nonpilots. Aviat Space Environ Med 84:726–729

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von Stumm S (2013) Intelligence, gender, and assessment method affect the accuracy of self-estimated intelligence. Br J Psychol. doi:10.1111/bjop.12031

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch DC, West RL (1995) Self-efficacy and mastery: its application to issues of environmental control, cognition, and aging. Dev Rev 15:150–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelinski EM, Gilewski MJ, Anthony-Bergstone CR (1990) The memory functioning questionnaire: concurrent validity with memory performance and self-reported memory failures. Psychol Aging 5:388–399

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Cristina Canovi and Matteo Migliori for their help in collecting data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Raffaella Nori.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nori, R., Piccardi, L. I believe I’m good at orienting myself… But is that true?. Cogn Process 16, 301–307 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0655-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0655-3

Keywords

Navigation