Abstract
Humans typically exhibit a tendency to follow the gaze of conspecifics, a social attention behaviour known as gaze cueing. Here, we addressed whether episodically learned social knowledge about the behaviours performed by the individual bearing the gaze can influence this phenomenon. In a learning phase, different faces were systematically associated with either positive or negative behaviours. The same faces were then used as stimuli in a gaze-cueing task. The results showed that faces associated with antisocial norm-violating behaviours triggered stronger gaze-cueing effects as compared to faces associated with sociable behaviours. Importantly, this was especially evident for participants who perceived the presented norm-violating behaviours as far more negative as compared to positive behaviours. These findings suggest that reflexive attentional responses can be affected by our appraisal of the valence of the behaviours of individuals around us.
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-016-0785-2/MediaObjects/10339_2016_785_Fig1_HTML.gif)
![](http://media.springernature.com/m312/springer-static/image/art%3A10.1007%2Fs10339-016-0785-2/MediaObjects/10339_2016_785_Fig2_HTML.gif)
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Notes
SOA is a crucial variable to sample attention allocation over time in spatial cueing tasks (see Chica et al. 2014). As for the gaze cueing paradigm, there is evidence suggesting that the magnitude of the gaze-cueing effect can be stronger at longer SOAs, a finding reported in both healthy individuals (e.g., Dalmaso et al. 2016; Driver et al. 1999; Frischen and Tipper 2006) and in clinical populations (e.g., Dalmaso et al. 2013). However, when social variables are manipulated, the effects exerted by these social manipulations on gaze cueing seem to be more evident at shorter SOAs (e.g., Dalmaso et al. 2014; Jones et al. 2010).
References
Berthoz S, Armony JL, Blair RJR, Dolan RJ (2002) An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain 125:1696–1708. doi:10.1093/brain/awf190
Carraro L, Dalmaso M, Castelli L, Galfano G (2015) The politics of attention contextualized: gaze but not arrow cuing of attention is moderated by political temperament. Cogn Process 16:309–314. doi:10.1007/s10339-015-0661-5
Cazzato V, Liuzza MT, Macaluso E, Caprara GV, Aglioti SM (2015) The attracting power of the gaze of politicians is modulated by the personality and ideological attitude of their voters: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Eur J Neurosci 42:2534–2545. doi:10.1111/ejn.13038
Chen Y, Zhao Y (2015) Intergroup threat gates social attention in humans. Biol Lett 11:20141055. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.1055
Chica AB, Martín-Arévalo E, Botta F, Lupiánez J (2014) The Spatial Orienting paradigm: how to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 40:35–51. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.002
Ciardo F, Marino BFM, Actis-Grosso R, Rossetti A, Ricciardelli P (2014) Face age modulates gaze following in young adults. Sci Rep 4:4746. doi:10.1038/srep04746
Coull A, Yzerbyt VY, Castano E, Paladino MP, Leemans V (2001) Protecting the ingroup: motivated allocation of cognitive resources in the presence of threatening ingroup members. Group Process Intergr Relat 4:327–339. doi:10.1177/1368430201004004003
Dalmaso M, Pavan G, Castelli L, Galfano G (2012) Social status gates social attention in humans. Biol Lett 8:450–452. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0881
Dalmaso M, Galfano G, Tarqui L, Forti B, Castelli L (2013) Is social attention impaired in schizophrenia? Gaze but not pointing gestures is associated with spatial attention deficits. Neuropsychology 27:608–613. doi:10.1037/a0033518
Dalmaso M, Galfano G, Coricelli C, Castelli L (2014) Temporal dynamics underlying the modulation of social status on social attention. PLoS ONE 9:e93139. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0093139
Dalmaso M, Galfano G, Castelli L (2015) The impact of same- and other-race gaze distractors on the control of saccadic eye movements. Perception 44:1020–1028. doi:10.1177/0301006615594936
Dalmaso M, Edwards SG, Bayliss AP (2016) Re-encountering individuals who previously engaged in joint attention modulates subsequent gaze cueing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 42:271–284. doi:10.1037/xlm0000159
Deaner RO, Shepherd SV, Platt ML (2007) Familiarity accentuates gaze cuing in women but not men. Biol Lett 3:64–67. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0564
Dodd MD, Hibbing JR, Smith KB (2011) The politics of attention: gaze-cuing effects are moderated by political temperament. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:24–29. doi:10.3758/s13414-010-0001-x
Dodd MD, Hibbing JR, Smith KB (2016) The politics of attention: differences in visual cognition between liberals and conservatives. Psychol Learn Motiv 65:277–309. doi:10.1016/bs.plm.2016.04.003
Driver J, Davis G, Ricciardelli P, Kidd P, Maxwell E, Baron-Cohen S (1999) Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Vis Cogn 6:509–540. doi:10.1080/135062899394920
Fox E, Mathews A, Calder AJ, Yiend J (2007) Anxiety and sensitivity to gaze direction in emotionally expressive faces. Emotion 7:478–486. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.478
Frischen A, Tipper SP (2006) Long-term gaze cueing effects: evidence for retrieval of prior states of attention from memory. Vis Cogn 14:351–364. doi:10.1080/13506280544000192
Frischen A, Bayliss AP, Tipper SP (2007) Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychol Bull 133:694–724. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694
Graham J, Nosek BA, Haidt J, Iyer R, Koleva S, Ditto PH (2011) Mapping the moral domain. J Pers Soc Psychol 101:366–385. doi:10.1037/a0021847
Hungr CJ, Hunt AR (2012) Physical self-similarity enhances the gaze-cueing effect. Q J Exp Psychol 65:1250–1259. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.690769
Jaeger TF (2008) Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. J Mem Lang 59:434–446. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Main JC, Little AC, Welling LL, Feinberg DR, Tiddeman BP (2010) Facial cues of dominance modulate the short-term gaze-cuing effect in human observers. Proc R Soc B 277:617–624. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1575
Kuhn G, Pickering A, Cole GG (2016) “Rare” emotive faces and attentional orienting. Emotion 16:1–5. doi:10.1037/emo0000050
Liuzza MT, Cazzato V, Vecchione M, Crostella F, Caprara GV, Aglioti SM (2011) Follow my eyes: the gaze of politicians reflexively captures the gaze of ingroup voters. PLoS ONE 6:e25117. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025117
Micheletta J, Waller BM (2012) Friendship affects gaze following in a tolerant species of macaque, Macaca nigra. Anim Behav 83:459–467. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.018
Pavan G, Dalmaso M, Galfano G, Castelli L (2011) Racial group membership is associated to gaze-mediated orienting in Italy. PLoS ONE 6:e25608. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025608
Porciello G, Holmes B, Liuzza MT, Crostella F, Aglioti SM, Bufalari I (2014) Interpersonal multisensory stimulation reduces the overwhelming distracting power of self-gaze: psychophysical evidence for ‘engazement’. Sci Rep 4:6669. doi:10.1038/srep06669
Porciello G, Liuzza MT, Minio-Paluello I, Caprara GV, Aglioti SM (2016) Fortunes and misfortunes of political leaders reflected in the eyes of their electors. Exp Brain Res 234:733–740. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4496-1
Reese G, Steffens MC, Jonas KJ (2013) When black sheep make us think: information processing and devaluation of in- and outgroup norm deviants. Soc Cogn 31:482–503. doi:10.1521/soco_2012_1005
Shepherd SV, Deaner RO, Platt ML (2006) Social status gates social attention in monkeys. Curr Biol 16:119–120. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.013
Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U (2011) False-positive psychology undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol Sci 22:1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632
Süßenbach F, Schönbrodt F (2014) Not afraid to trust you: trustworthiness moderates gaze cueing but not in highly anxious participants. J Cogn Psychol 26:670–678. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.945457
van Kleef GA, Wanders F, Stamkou E, Homan AC (2015) The social dynamics of breaking the rules: antecedents and consequences of norm-violating behavior. Curr Opin Psychol 6:25–31. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.013
van Leeuwen F, Park JH, Penton-Voak IS (2012) Another fundamental social category? Spontaneous categorization of people who uphold or violate moral norms. J Exp Soc Psychol 48:1385–1388. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.004
Wagenmakers EJ (2007) A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychon Bull Rev 14:779–804. doi:10.1007/s10339-015-0661-5
Wojciszke B (2005) Morality and competence in person-and self-perception. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 16:155–188. doi:10.1080/10463280500229619
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by MIUR (Futuro in Ricerca 2012, Grants RBFR12F0BD and RBFR128CR6).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Handling Editor: Bruno Laeng (University of Oslo).
Reviewers: Anna Pecchinenda (Sapienza University of Rome) and Olga Chelnokova (University of Oslo).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Carraro, L., Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L. et al. The appeal of the devil’s eye: social evaluation affects social attention. Cogn Process 18, 97–103 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0785-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0785-2