Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Using a cognitive architecture with a physiological substrate to represent effects of a psychological stressor on cognition

  • Manuscript
  • Published:
Computational and Mathematical Organization Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Adding a physiological representation to a cognitive architecture offers an attractive approach to modeling the effects of stress on cognition. We introduce ACT-R/Φ, an extended version of the ACT-R cognitive architecture that includes an integrative model of physiology. The extension allows the representation of how physiology and cognition interact. This substrate was used to represent potential effects of a startle response and task-based stress during a mental arithmetic (subtraction) task. We compare predictions from two models loaded into the new hybrid architecture to models previously developed within ACT-R. General behavior differed between models in that the ACT-R/Φ models had dynamic declarative memory noise over the course of the task based on varying epinephrine levels. They attempted more subtractions but were less accurate; this more closely matched human performance than the previous ACT-R models. Using ACT-R/Φ allows a more tractable integration of current physiological and cognitive perspectives on stress. ACT-R/Φ also permits further exploration of the interaction between cognition and physiology, and the emergent effects on behavior caused by the interaction among physiological subsystems. This extension is useful for anyone exploring how the human mind can occur in and be influenced by the physical universe.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.

Notes

  1. Here, by biomathematical models, we mean mathematical models that provide a quantitative representation of how some biological process affects the state of a cognitive system, in this case, the model represents alertness (see Gunzelmann et al. 2009, for further discussion).

  2. As implied by Ritter et al. (2007) and explicitly discussed by Ritter et al. (2012).

  3. A newer version of the architecture has been developed to also include a representation of affect/emotion (Dancy, 2013). A version with just the physio module was used for this work.

  4. More information on that model and project is located at http://acs.ist.psu.edu/ACT-R_AC/.

  5. The aversive speech sound is presented at the 2 min point in every block.

  6. Reviews are available on the underlying physiology of the stress response (e.g., Charmandari et al. 2005; Joëls and Baram, 2009) and the underlying physiology of internal (e.g., Kemeny and Shestyuk 2008) and external (e.g., Öhman 2008) causes and effects of the stress response.

  7. This is a simple approximation to an appraisal mechanism.

  8. Our models (Eqs. 1 and 2) had a standard error of the mean (SEM) of 0.708 and 0.920 (respectively). While these SEMs are higher than that reported as an example in Ritter et al. (2011), our much higher run cost (~2*real-time) modified our SEM threshold.

  9. This time interval was chosen because this is how often physiological values were updated in ACT-RΦ (the :phys-delay parameter). Thus, :ans values automatically changed the moment physiology changed.

  10. The syllable-rate parameter controls the time it takes the model to articulate each syllable in a text string.

  11. Base-level constant is a parameter used during the ACT-R memory retrieval process.

  12. As a reminder, this model has a varying declarative memory noise value due to physiological modulation.

  13. See Byrne et al. (2004) and Gunzelmann et al. (2011) for counterexamples.

References

  • Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A, De Vries P (2001) On the psychology of drinking: being thirsty and perceptually ready. Br J Psychol 92(4):631–642

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR (2007) How can the human mind occur in the physical universe?. OUP, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR, Gluck K (2001) What role do cognitive architectures play in intelligent tutoring systems. In: Carver SM, Klahr D (eds) Cognition & instruction: twenty-five years of progress. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 227–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR, Fincham JM, Qin Y, Stocco A (2008) A central circuit of the mind. Trends Cogn Sci 12(4):136–143

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bach J (2009) Principles of synthetic intelligence: PSI: an architecture of motivated cognition. OUP, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bartl C, Dörner D (1998) PSI: A theory of the integration of cognition, emotion and motivation. In: Proceedings of the 2nd european conference on cognitive modelling. Thrumpton, Nottingham, pp 66–73

  • Byrne MD (2013) How many times should a stochastic model be run? An approach based on confidence intervals. In: Proceedings of the 12th International conference on cognitive modeling, Ottawa

  • Byrne MD, Kirlik A, Fleetwood MD, Huss DG, Kosorukoff A, Lin R, Fick CS (2004) A closed-loop, ACT-R approach to modeling approach and landing with and without synthetic vision system (SVS) technology. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society annual meeting, pp 2111–2115

  • Cahill L, Alkire MT (2003) Epinephrine enhancement of human memory consolidation: interaction with arousal at encoding. Neurobiol Learn Mem 79(2):194–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charmandari E, Tsigos C, Chrousos G (2005) Endocrinology of the stress response. Annu Rev Physiol 67(1):259–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dancy CL (2013) ACT-RΦ: A cognitive architecture with physiology and affect. Biol Inspir Cogn Archit 6(1):40–45

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincham JM, Anderson JR, Betts SA, Ferris JL (2010) Using neural imaging and cognitive modeling to infer mental states while using an intelligent tutoring system. In: Proceedings of the third international conference on educational data mining (EDM2010)

  • Gluck K, Pew RW (eds) (2005) Modeling human behavior with integrated cognitive architectures: comparison, evaluation, and validation, 1st edn. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunzelmann G, Gross JB, Gluck KA, Dinges DF (2009) Sleep deprivation and sustained attention performance: integrating mathematical and cognitive modeling. Cogn Sci 33(5):880–910

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunzelmann G, Moore RL Jr, Salvucci DD, Gluck KA (2011) Sleep loss and driver performance: quantitative predictions with zero free parameters. Cogn Syst Res 12(2):154–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunzelmann G, Gluck KA, Moore RL Jr, Dinges DF (2012) Diminished access to declarative knowledge with sleep deprivation. Cogn Syst Res 13(1):1–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guyton AC, Coleman TG, Granger HJ (1972) Circulation: overall regulation. Annu Rev Physiol 34(1):13–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hester RL, Brown AJ, Husband L, Iliescu R, Pruett D, Summers R, Coleman TG (2011a) HumMod: a modeling environment for the simulation of integrative human physiology. Front Physiol. doi:10.3389/fphys.2011.00012

    Google Scholar 

  • Hester RL, Iliescu R, Summers R, Coleman TG (2011b) Systems biology and integrative physiological modelling. J Physiol 589(5):1053–1060

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudlicka E, Mcneese MD (2002) Assessment of user affective and belief states for interface adaptation: application to an Air Force pilot task. User Model User-Adap Interact 12(1):1–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joëls M, Baram TZ (2009) The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6):459–466. doi:10.1038/nrn2632

    Google Scholar 

  • Joëls M, Pu Z, Wiegert O, Oitzl MS, Krugers HJ (2006) Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends Cogn Sci 10(4):152–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemeny ME, Shestyuk A (2008) Emotions, the neuroendocrine and immune systems, and health. In: Lewis MS, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF (eds) Handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 661–675

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, Hellhammer DH (1993) The “Trier social stress test”: a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28(1–2):76–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein LC, Bennett JM, Whetzel CA, Granger DA, Ritter FE (2010) Caffeine and stress alter salivary α-amylase activity in young men. Hum Psychopharmacol 25(5):359–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebiere C, Anderson JR, Bothell D (2001) Multi-tasking and cognitive workload in an ACT-R model of a simplified air traffic control task. In: Proceedings of the 10th conference on computer generated forces and behavior representation

  • Marsella S, Gratch J (2009) EMA: a process model of appraisal dynamics. Cogn Syst Res 10(1):70–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsella S, Gratch J, Petta P (2010) Computational models of emotion. In: Scherer KR, Banziger T, Roesch EB (eds) A blueprint for an affectively competent agent: crossfertilization between emotion psychology, affective neuroscience, and affective computing. OUP, New York, pp 21–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyashita T, Williams CL (2006) Epinephrine administration increases neural impulses propagated along the vagus nerve: role of peripheral β-adrenergic receptors. Neurobiol Learn Mem 85(2):116–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hyare H, Lee S (1998) Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behav Res Ther 36(2):227–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montani J-P, Van Vliet BN (2009) Understanding the contribution of Guyton’s large circulatory model to long-term control of arterial pressure. Exp Physiol 94(4):382–388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan JH, Morgan GP, Ritter FE (2010) A preliminary model of participation for small groups. Comput Math Organ Theory 16(3):246–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman A (2008) Fear and anxiety: overlaps and dissociations. In: Lewis MS, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF (eds) Handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 709–729

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew RW, Mavor AS (eds) (1998) Modeling human and organizational behavior: application to military simulations. National Academies Press, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao AS, Georgeff MP (1995) BDI-agents: from theory to practice. In: Proceedings of the first international conference on multi-agents-systems. Menlo Park, pp 312–319

  • Ritter FE, Reifers AL, Klein LC, Schoelles MJ (2007) Lessons from defining theories of stress. In: Gray WD (ed) Integrated models of cognitive systems. OUP, New York, pp 254–262

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter FE, Kase SE, Klein LC, Bennett J, Schoelles M (2009) Fitting a model to behavior tells us what changes cognitively when under stress and with caffeine. In: Proceedings of the biologically inspired cognitive architectures symposium at the AAAI fall symposium series. Keynote presentation, Washington, pp 109–115

  • Ritter FE, Schoelles MJ, Quigley KS, Klein LC (2011) Determining the number of simulation runs: treating simulations as theories by not sampling their behavior. In: Narayanan S, Rothrock L (eds) Human-in-the-loop simulations: methods and practice. Springer, London, pp 97–116

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter FE, Bittner JL, Kase SE, Evertsz R, Pedrotti M, Busetta P (2012) CoJACK: a high-level cognitive architecture with demonstrations of moderators, variability, and implications for situation awareness. Biol Inspir Cogn Archit 1:2–13

    Google Scholar 

  • Sara SJ (2009) The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(3):211–223. doi:10.1038/nrn2573

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman BG, Cornwell J, O’Brien K (2004) Human performance simulation. In: Ness JW, Ritzer DR, Tepe V (eds) Metrics and methods in human performance research toward individual and small unit simulation. Human Systems Information Analysis Center, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP (2009) Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6):397–409. doi:10.1038/nrn2647

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright ND, Hodgson K, Fleming SM, Symmonds M, Guitart-Masip M, Dolan RJ (2012) Human responses to unfairness with primary rewards and their biological limits. Sci Rep. doi:10.1038/srep00593

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes RM, Dodson JD (1908) The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18(5):459–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao C, Kaulakis R, Morgan JH, Hiam JW, Ritter FE, Sanford JP, Morgan GP (This issue) Building social networks out of cognitive blocks: factors of interest in agent-based socio-cognitive simulations. Comput Math Organ Theory

Download references

Acknowledgments

The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of Defense or the U.S. Government. The authors thank Sue Kase for providing the original ACT-R serial subtraction model and Robert Hester for useful discussions related to the HumMod simulation system. The authors also thank Kuo-Chuan (Martin) Yeh, Alex Ororbia, and several anonymous reviewers for many very useful comments. This work was partially funded by the Bunton-Waller Fellowship Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher L. Dancy.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dancy, C.L., Ritter, F.E., Berry, K.A. et al. Using a cognitive architecture with a physiological substrate to represent effects of a psychological stressor on cognition. Comput Math Organ Theory 21, 90–114 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10588-014-9178-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10588-014-9178-1

Keywords