Abstract
We propose, and test, a model of online media platform users’ decisions to act on, and share, received information. Specifically, we focus on how mental representations of message content drive its spread. Our model is based on fuzzy-trace theory (FTT), a leading theory of decision under risk. Per FTT, online content is mentally represented in two ways: verbatim (objective, but decontextualized, facts), and gist (subjective, but meaningful, interpretation). Although encoded in parallel, gist tends to drive behaviors more strongly than verbatim representations for most individuals. Our model uses factors derived from FTT to make predictions regarding which content is more likely to be shared, namely: (a) different levels of mental representation, (b) the motivational content of a message, (c) difficulty of information processing (e.g., the ease with which a given message may be comprehended and, therefore, its gist extracted), and (d) social values.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning.Notes
Ten threats to global health in 2019. https://www.who.int/emergencies/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019. Accessed 14 Mar 2019.
Corresponding tweet ids may be found at https://github.com/broniatowski/Illuminate-and-Motivate.
About verified accounts. https://help.twitter.com/en/managing-your-account/about-twitter-verified-accounts. Accessed 15 Mar 2019.
References
Akaike H (1976) Canonical correlation analysis of time series and the use of an information criterion. In: Mathematics in science and engineering, vol 126, Elsevier, pp 27–96
Bakshy E, Messing S, Adamic LA (2015) Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on facebook. Science 348(6239):1130–1132
Bansal S (2018) textstat:memo: python package to calculate readability statistics of a text object—paragraphs, sentences, articles.https://github.com/shivam5992/textstat. Accessed 16 June 2014
Berger J, Milkman KL (2012) What makes online content viral? J Mark Res 49(2):192–205
Betsch C, Brewer NT, Brocard P, Davies P, Gaissmaier W, Haase N, Leask J, Renkewitz F, Renner B, Reyna VF et al (2012) Opportunities and challenges of Web 2.0 for vaccination decisions. Vaccine 30(25):3727–3733
Blei DM, Ng AY, Jordan MI (2003) Latent dirichlet allocation. J Mach Learn Res 3:993–1022
Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1999) Affective norms for english words (anew). The NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida, Gainesville
Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Rothman AJ, Leask J, Kempe A (2017) Increasing vaccination: putting psychological science into action. Psychol Sci Public Interest 18(3):149–207
Broniatowski D, Reyna V (2018) A formal model of fuzzy-trace theory. Decision 5(4):205–252
Broniatowski DA, Klein EY, Reyna VF (2015) Germs are germs, and why not take a risk? Patients’ expectations for prescribing antibiotics in an Inner-City Emergency Department. Med Decis Mak 35(1):60–67
Broniatowski DA, Hilyard KM, Dredze M (2016) Effective vaccine communication during the disneyland measles outbreak. Vaccine 34(28):3225–3228 7
Broniatowski DA, Jamison AM, Qi S, AlKulaib L, Chen T, Benton A, Quinn SC, Dredze M (2018) Weaponized health communication: twitter bots and Russian trolls amplify the vaccine debate. Am J Public Health 108(10):1378–1384
Busemeyer JR, Wang YM (2000) Model comparisons and model selections based on generalization criterion methodology. J Math Psychol 44(1):171–189
Buttenheim AM, Asch DA (2016) Leveraging behavioral insights to promote vaccine acceptance: one year after disneyland. JAMA Pediatr 170(7):635–636
Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Feng Kao C (1984) The efficient assessment of need for cognition. J Personal Assess 48(3):306–307
Cacioppo JT, Feinstein JA, Jarvis WBG (1996) Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychol Bull 119(2):197
Centola D (2010) The spread of behavior in an online social network experiment. Science 329(5996):1194–1197
Centola D (2011) An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of health behavior. Science 334(6060):1269–1272
Chang J, Gerrish S, Wang C, Boyd-Graber JL, Blei DM (2009) Reading tea leaves: how humans interpret topic models. In: Advances in neural information processing systems, pp 288–296
Chen T, Dredze M (2018) Vaccine images on twitter: analysis of what images are shared. J Med Internet Res 20(4):e130
Cheng J, Adamic L, Dow PA, Kleinberg JM, Leskovec J (2014) Can cascades be predicted? In: Proceedings of the 23rd international conference on World wide web, ACM, pp 925–936
Chou WYS, Oh A, Klein WM (2018) Addressing health-related misinformation on social media. Jama 320(23):2417–2418
Cokely ET, Kelley CM (2009) Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: a protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgm Decis Mak 4(1):20–33
Cowling BJ, Fang VJ, Nishiura H, Chan KH, Ng S, Ip DK, Chiu SS, Leung GM, Peiris JM (2012) Increased risk of noninfluenza respiratory virus infections associated with receipt of inactivated influenza vaccine. Clin Infect Dis 54(12):1778–1783
Curseu PL (2006) Need for cognition and rationality in decision-making. Stud Psychol 48(2):141
Diehl JJ, Bennetto L, Young EC (2006) Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34(1):83–98
Dredze M, Broniatowski DA, Hilyard KM (2016a) Zika vaccine misconceptions: a social media analysis. Vaccine 34(30):3441–3442
Dredze M, Broniatowski DA, Smith MC, Hilyard KM (2016b) Understanding vaccine refusal: why we need social media now. AmJ Prev Med 50(4):550–552
Dredze M, Wood-Doughty Z, Quinn SC, Broniatowski DA (2017) Vaccine opponents’ use of twitter during the 2016 us presidential election: implications for practice and policy. Vaccine 35(36):4670–4672
Frederick S (2005) Cognitive reflection and decision making. J Econ Perspect 19(4):25–42
Fukukura J, Ferguson MJ, Fujita K (2013) Psychological distance can improve decision making under information overload via gist memory. J Exp Psychol 142(3):658
Galván A (2017) The neuroscience of adolescence, 1st edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, New York
Gernsbacher MA, Varner KR, Faust ME (1990) Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. J Exp Psychol 16(3):430
Gernsbacher MA (1996) The structure-building framework: what it is, what it might also be, and why. In: Britton BK, Graesser AC (eds) Models of understanding text. Psychology Press, New York, NY, pp 289–311
Golbeck J, Robles C, Edmondson M, Turner K (2011) Predicting personality from twitter. In: 2011 IEEE Third International conference on privacy, security, risk and trust (PASSAT) and 2011 IEEE Third International conference on social computing (SocialCom), IEEE, pp 149–156
Goldman SR, McCarthy KS, Burkett C (2015) Interpretive inferences in literature. In: Inferences during reading, p 386
Granovetter M, Soong R (1983) Threshold models of diffusion and collective behavior. J Math Sociol 9(3):165–179
Griffiths TL, Steyvers M, Tenenbaum JB (2007) Topics in semantic representation. Psychol Rev 114(2):211
Grinberg N, Joseph K, Friedland L, Swire-Thompson B, Lazer D (2019) Fake news on twitter during the 2016 us presidential election. Science 363(6425):374–378
Hsee CK, Rottenstreich Y (2004) Music, pandas, and muggers: on the affective psychology of value. J Exp Psychol 133(1):23
Jamison AM, Broniatowski D, Quinn SC (2019) Malicious actors on twitter: a guide for public health researchers. Am J Public Health 109:688–692
Kintsch W (1974) The representation of meaning in memory. Lawrence Erlbnum Associates, Hillsdale
Klein EY, Martinez EM, May L, Saheed M, Reyna V, Broniatowski DA (2017) Categorical risk perception drives variability in antibiotic prescribing in the Emergency Department: a mixed methods observational study. J Gen Intern Med 32(10):1083–1089
LaTour KA, LaTour MS, Brainerd C (2014) Fuzzy trace theory and “smart” false memories: implications for advertising. J Advert 43(1):3–17
LeBoeuf RA, Shafir E (2003) Deep thoughts and shallow frames: on the susceptibility to framing effects. J Behav Decis Mak 16(2):77–92
Liberali JM, Reyna VF, Furlan S, Stein LM, Pardo ST (2012) Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. J Behav Decis Mak 25(4):361–381
Linderholm T, Everson MG, van den Broek P, Mischinski M, Crittenden A, Samuels J (2000) Effects of causal text revisions on more- and less-skilled readers’ comprehension of easy and difficult texts. Cogn Instr 18(4):525–556
Mandler JM (1983) What a story is. Behav Brain Sci 6(04):603–604
Mohammad SM, Kiritchenko S (2015) Using hashtags to capture fine emotion categories from tweets. Comput Intell 31(2):301–326
Mohammad SM, Turney PD (2013) Crowdsourcing a word-emotion association lexicon. Comput Intell 29(3):436–465
Myrick JG (2015) Emotion regulation, procrastination, and watching cat videos online: who watches internet cats, why, and to what effect? Comput Hum Behav 52:168–176
Mnøsted B, Sapieżyński P, Ferrara E, Lehmann S (2017) Evidence of complex contagion of information in social media: an experiment using twitter bots. PLoS ONE 12(9):e0184148
Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, Blondel M, Prettenhofer P, Weiss R, Dubourg V, Vanderplas J, Passos A, Cournapeau D, Brucher M, Perrot M, Duchesnay E (2011) Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J Mach Learn Res 12:2825–2830
Pennington N, Hastie R (1991) A cognitive theory of juror decision making: the story model. Cardozo L Rev 13:519
Pennycook G, Rand DG (2018) Lazy, not biased: susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition 188:39–50
Pennycook G, Cheyne JA, Koehler DJ, Fugelsang JA (2016) Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition? Behav Res Methods 48(1):341–348
Pennycook G, Cannon TD, Rand DG (2018) Prior exposure increases perceived accuracy of fake news. J Exp Psychol 147:1865–1880
Perrin A (2015) Social media usage: 2005–2015
Peters E, Levin IP (2008) Dissecting the risky-choice framing effect: numeracy as an individual-difference factor in weighting risky and riskless options. Judgm Decis Mak 3(6):435–448
Peters E, Västfjäll D, Slovic P, Mertz CK, Mazzocco K, Dickert S (2006) Numeracy and decision making. Psychol Sci 17(5):407–413
Petrovic S, Osborne M, Lavrenko V (2011) RT to win! Predicting message propagation in twitter. ICWSM 11:586–589
Plutchik R (2001) The nature of emotions: human emotions have deep evolutionary roots, a fact that may explain their complexity and provide tools for clinical practice. Am Sci 89(4):344–350
Quercia D, Kosinski M, Stillwell D, Crowcroft J (2011) Our twitter profiles, our selves: predicting personality with twitter. In: 2011 IEEE Third International conference on privacy, security, risk and trust (PASSAT) and 2011 IEEE Third International conference on social computing (SocialCom), IEEE, pp 180–185
Quinn SC, Parmer J, Freimuth VS, Hilyard KM, Musa D, Kim KH (2013) Exploring communication, trust in government, and vaccination intention later in the 2009 H1n1 pandemic: results of a national survey. Biosecur Bioterror 11(2):96–106
Rapp DN, Pvd Broek, McMaster KL, Kendeou P, Espin CA (2007) Higher-order comprehension processes in struggling readers: a perspective for research and intervention. Sci Stud Read 11(4):289–312
Reese E, Haden CA, Baker-Ward L, Bauer P, Fivush R, Ornstein PA (2011) Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: a multidimensional model and coding method. J Cogn Dev 12(4):424–462
Reyna VF (2012) Risk perception and communication in vaccination decisions: a fuzzy-trace theory approach. Vaccine 30(25):3790–3797
Reyna VF, Adam MB (2003) Fuzzy-trace theory, risk communication, and product labeling in sexually transmitted diseases. Risk Anal 23(2):325–342
Reyna VF, Brainerd CJ (2008) Numeracy, ratio bias, and denominator neglect in judgments of risk and probability. Learn Individ Differ 18(1):89–107
Reyna VF, Lloyd FJ (2006) Physician decision making and cardiac risk: effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. J Exp Psychol 12(3):179
Reyna VF, Estrada SM, DeMarinis JA, Myers RM, Stanisz JM, Mills BA (2011) Neurobiological and memory models of risky decision making in adolescents versus young adults. J Exp Psychol 37(5):1125
Reyna VF, Corbin JC, Weldon RB, Brainerd CJ (2016) How fuzzy-trace theory predicts true and false memories for words, sentences, and narratives. J Appl Res Mem Cogn 5(1):1–9
Riddell A (2014) Lda: 0.3.2. 10.5281/zenodo.592664. https://zenodo.org/record/592664. Accessed 16 July 2018
Rivers SE, Reyna VF, Mills B (2008) Risk taking under the influence: a fuzzy-trace theory of emotion in adolescence. Dev Rev 28(1):107–144
Rogers EM (2010) Diffusion of innovations. Simon and Schuster, New York
Romero DM, Meeder B, Kleinberg J (2011) Differences in the mechanics of information diffusion across topics: idioms, political hashtags, and complex contagion on twitter. In: Proceedings of the 20th international conference on World wide web, ACM, pp 695–704
Schley DR, Peters E (2014) Assessing economic value symbolic-number mappings predict risky and riskless valuations. Psychol Sci 25:753–761
Shmueli G et al (2010) To explain or to predict? Stat Sci 25(3):289–310
Silverman C (2016) This analysis shows how viral fake election news stories outperformed real news on facebook. Retrieved February 15, 2017, from https://www.buzzfeed.com/craigsilverman/viral-fake-election-news-outperformed-real-news-on-facebook
Simon AF, Fagley NS, Halleran JG (2004) Decision framing: moderating effects of individual differences and cognitive processing. J Behav Decis Mak 17(2):77–93
Smith A, Anderson M (2018) Social media use in 2018. Pew Research Center 1
Stevens SS et al (1946) On the theory of scales of measurement. Science 103:677–680
Subrahmanian V, Azaria A, Durst S, Kagan V, Galstyan A, Lerman K, Zhu L, Ferrara E, Flammini A, Menczer F (2016) The darpa twitter bot challenge. Computer 49(6):38–46
Sundaram ME, McClure DL, VanWormer JJ, Friedrich TC, Meece JK, Belongia EA (2013) Influenza vaccination is not associated with detection of noninfluenza respiratory viruses in seasonal studies of influenza vaccine effectiveness. Clin Infect Dis 57(6):789–793
Swire B, Berinsky AJ, Lewandowsky S, Ecker UK (2017) Processing political misinformation: comprehending the trump phenomenon. R Soc Open Sci 4(3):160802
Trabasso T, Sperry LL (1985) Causal relatedness and importance of story events. J Mem Lang 24(5):595–611
Trabasso T, Secco T, Van Den Broek P (1984) Causal cohesion and story coherence. In: Mandl H, Stein NL, Trabasso T (eds) Learning and comprehension of text. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 83–110
Trope Y, Liberman N (2010) Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol Rev 117(2):440
Tsur O, Rappoport A (2012) What’s in a hashtag?: content based prediction of the spread of ideas in microblogging communities. In: Proceedings of the fifth ACM international conference on Web search and data mining, ACM, pp 643–652
Tversky A, Kahneman D (1981) The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211(4481):453–458
Van den Broek P (2010) Using texts in science education: cognitive processes and knowledge representation. Science 328(5977):453–456
van den Broek P, Helder A (2017) Cognitive processes in discourse comprehension: passive processes, reader-initiated processes, and evolving mental representations. Discourse Process 54:1–13
Vazquez MA (2016) Informe de Médicos de Pueblos Fumigados sobre Dengue-Zika y fumigaciones con venenos químicoshttp://alimentoyconciencia.com/informe-de-medicos-de-pueblos-fumigados-sobre-dengue-zika-y-fumigaciones-con-venenos-quimicos/. Accessed 06 Feb 2017
Vosoughi S, Roy D, Aral S (2018) The spread of true and false news online. Science 359(6380):1146–1151
Acknowledgement
Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences R01GM114771 to the first author.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendix
Appendix
This Appendix contains details of our model fitting methodologies.
1.1 Linear regression
We first identified 1388 tweets that had been retweeted at least once. These tweets were used for subsequent analyses.
1.1.1 Bidirectional stepwise elimination
We implemented Bidirectional stepwise elimination using the R Project for Statistical Computing version 3.5.1. Specifically, we used the step function in R. The starting model contained main effects for the following features:
-
1.
All 50 topic proportions (logit transformed)
-
2.
Dummy variables for media and user verification
-
3.
All three PCA dimensions for text complexity in Table 3
-
4.
All three PCA dimensions for emotion in Table 4
-
5.
tweet sentiment (positive, negative, or neutral)
Given this initial model, the step function successively added interaction terms (specifically between tweet sentiment, topic proportion, and text complexity) and removed other terms already in the model until AIC reached a local minimum. The resulting model is shown in Table 9.
1.1.2 LASSO regression
In parallel, we used LASSO regression to fit a model with the same predictors as above (including two- and three-way interactions between topic proportions, topic sentiment, and readability). We held out one third of our 1,388 datapoints, keeping the remaining data for training and test. LASSO regression was implemented using the scikit-learn package in Python 2.7. Specifically, we used the LassoCV function with 10,000 iterations and with eps=1e-4. The resulting model is shown in Table 10.
1.1.3 Model concordance
Although the bidirectional and LASSO models have several similarities, they also have many differences. We therefore fit a second pair of OLS models using only those covariates that appeared in both the bidirectional and LASSO models (see Table 11). Finally, we removed all covariates that were not significant after multiple comparisons using the Holm-Bonferroni procedure to generate our final model, shown in Table 5.
1.2 Logistic regression
We next compared the 1388 tweets that had been retweeted at least once to the remaining tweets in our sample—i.e., those that had not been retweeted.
1.2.1 Bidirectional stepwise elimination
We once again implemented Bidirectional stepwise elimination using the step function in R. The starting model contained the same main effects and scope as in the linear regression model. The resulting model is shown in Table 12.
1.2.2 \(L_1\)-norm regularization
In parallel, we used \(L_1\)-norm regularization to fit a model with the same predictors as above (including two- and three-way interactions between topic proportions, topic sentiment, and readability). We retained the 1,388 datapoints that had been retweeted at least once and randomly sampled another 1,388 datapoints from the remaining data to account for class imbalance. We held out one third of this combined dataset, keeping the remaining data for training and test. Logistic regression with \(L_1\)-norm regularization was implemented using the scikit-learn package in Python 2.7. Specifically, we used the LogisticRegressionCV function with 4,000 iterations and the liblinear solver. The resulting model is shown in Table 13.
1.2.3 Model concordance
We once again resolved differences between the two models by fitting a second pair of logistic regression models using only those covariates that appeared in both the bidirectional and \(L_1\)-norm models (see Table 14). Finally, we removed all covariates that were not significant after multiple comparisons using the Holm-Bonferroni procedure to generate our final model, shown in Table 7.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Broniatowski, D.A., Reyna, V.F. To illuminate and motivate: a fuzzy-trace model of the spread of information online. Comput Math Organ Theory 26, 431–464 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10588-019-09297-2
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10588-019-09297-2