Skip to main content
Log in

From Intersubjectivity to Group Cognition

  • Published:
Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW) Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The question of how it is possible for people to understand each other has been a controversial theme throughout the recent history of philosophy. It is a foundational issue for the social sciences, in which researchers try to understand the behaviors and statements of other people. It is of particular relevance to CSCW and CSCL, where participants have to understand, work with and learn with each other. Philosophers have posed the issue of how an individual can understand another and how a small group or community can have a joint understanding, shared intentionality or we-awareness. Studies of CSCW not only adopt insights from the philosophy of intersubjectivity to ground their methodology, they also contribute to the analysis of how intersubjectivity is established in concrete settings, including in virtual environments. Similarly, CSCL research can investigate how groups of people learn to construct intersubjective understandings in both traditional and technologically enhanced interactions. While classical phenomenology of intersubjectivity started from the cognitions of a solitary mind, the notion of intersubjectivity has subsequently shifted to a more social view. Recent studies of intersubjectivity suggest a structure of group cognition, which can provide a foundation for collaboration in work and learning that incorporates but transcends individual cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The masculine pronoun is used throughout this paper to refer to people of all genders, in keeping with traditional English grammar and philosophic usage.

  2. The notion of intentionality was first developed by Husserl’s teacher, Brentano (1874). As the lead article explains, it means that consciousness is always consciousness of something, always directed at something.

  3. Evolutionary development of mirror neurons and increased brain structure on the biological level may have accompanied and facilitated this increased sense of mutuality on the cultural level as a competitive advantage (Gallese and Lakoff 2005), but see also (Hickok 2014).

References

  • Adorno, T. W. (1964/1973). The jargon of authenticity. Evanston, IL: Northwestern U. Press.book series.

  • Brentano, F. (1874). Psychologie vom empirischen standpunkte [psychology from an empirical standpoint]. Leipzig, Germanybook series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. and S. Brennan (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. Resnick, J. Levine & S. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially-shared cognition. (pp. 127–149). Washington, DC: APA.

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little Brown and Company.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1633/1999). Discourse on method and meditations on first philosophy. New York, NY: Hackett.book series.

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dourish, P. (2001). Where the action is: The foundations of embodied interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. (1992). What computers still can’t do: A critique of artificial reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2010). Husserl, intersubjectivity, and anthropology. Anthropological Theory. 10(1–2), 16–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehn, P. (1988). Work-oriented design of computer artifacts. Stockholm, Sweden: Arbetslivscentrum.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emirbayer, M. and A. Mische (1998). What is agency? American Journal of Sociology. 103(4), 962–1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. and G.  Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 21(3–4), 455–479.

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (1971). Labor and interaction: Remarks on Hegel’s Jena philosophy of mind. In Theory and practice. (pp. 142–169). Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

  • Habermas, J. (1971/2001). On the pragmatics of interaction: Preliminary studies in the theory of communicative action (B. Fultner, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.book series.

  • Hegel, G. W. F. (1807/1967). Phenomenology of spirit (J. B. Baillie, Trans.). New York, NY: Harper & Row.book series.

  • Heidegger, M. (1927/1996). Being and time: A translation of Sein und Zeit (J. Stambaugh, Trans.). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.book series.

  • Heidegger, M. (1959/1971). On the way to language. New York, NY: Harper & Row.book series.

  • Hickok, G. (2014). The myth of mirror neurons: The real neuroscience of communication and cognition. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1929/1960). Cartesian meditations: An introduction to phenomenology (D. Cairns, Trans.). The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.book series.

  • Hutchins, E. (1996). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1787/1999). Critique of pure reason. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.book series.

  • Kuhn, T. (1972). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1990). Drawing things together. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1992). Where are the missing masses? The sociology of a few mundane artifacts. In W. E. Bijker & J. Law (Eds.), Shaping technology/building society. (pp. 225–227). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (2013). An inquiry into modes of existence: An anthropology of the modern (C. Porter, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.book series.

  • Linell, P. (2014). Interactivities, intersubjectivities and language: On dialogism and phenomenology. Language and Dialogue. 4(2), 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (1858/1939). Grundrisse der Kritik der politischen Oekonomie (Rohentwurf). Frankfurt, Germany: Europische Verlagsanstalt.book series.

  • Marx, K. (1867/1976). Capital (B. Fowkes, Trans. Vol. I). New York, NY: Vintage.book series.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/2002). The phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans. 2 ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.book series.

  • Plato. (340 BCE/1941). The republic (F. Cornford, Trans.). London, UK: Oxford University Press.book series.

  • Rommetveit, R. (2003). On the role of “a psychology of the second person” in studies of meaning, language and mind. Mind, Culture and Activity. vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 205–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. (1991). Conversation analysis and socially shared cognition. In L. Resnick, J. Levine & S. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially-shared cognition. (pp. 150–171). Washington, DC: APA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1932). Der sinnhafte aufbau der sozialen welt: Eine einleitung in die verstehende soziologie. Vienna, Austria: Springer.book series.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1932/1967). Phenomenology of the social world (F. Lehnert, Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.book series.

  • Seddon, C. (2014). Humans: From the beginning: From the first apes to the first cities. Kindle, ebook: Glanville Publications.book series.

  • Stahl, G. (1975). Marxian hermeneutics and Heideggerian social theory: Interpreting and transforming our world. Unpublished Dissertation, Ph.D., Department of Philosophy, Northwestern University. Evanston, IL. Web: http://gerrystahl.net/elibrary/marx.

  • Stahl, G. (2006). Group cognition: Computer support for building collaborative knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Acting with technology book series. 510 pages. Web: http://GerryStahl.net/elibrary/gc. Doi: http://mitpress.mit.edu/books/group-cognition.

  • Stahl, G. (2009). Studying virtual math teams. New York, NY: Springer. Computer-supported collaborative learning book series. 626 pages. Web: http://GerryStahl.net/elibrary/svmt. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0228-3.

  • Stahl, G. (2013). Translating Euclid: Designing a human-centered mathematics. San Rafael, CA: Morgan & Claypool Publishers. Synthesis lectures on human-centered informatics book series. 221 pages. Web: http://GerryStahl.net/elibrary/euclid. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2200/S00492ED1V01Y201303HCI017.

  • Stahl, G. (2015). Constructing dynamic triangles together: The development of mathematical group cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Learning in doing: Social, cognitive and computational perspectives book series. 250 pages. Web: http://GerryStahl.net/elibrary/analysis.

  • Suchman, L. A. (2007). Human-machine reconfigurations: Plans and situated actions (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.book series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenenberg, J., Roth, W.-M., & Socha, D. (2016). From I-awareness to we-awareness in CSCW. Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW). vol. 25, no. 4-5.

  • Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge: MA: Harvard University Press.book series

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1930/1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.book series.

  • Vygotsky, L. (1934/1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.book series.

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1991). A sociocultural approach to socially shared cognition. In L. Resnick, J. Levine & S. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially-shared cognition. (pp. 85–100). Washington, DC: APA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York, NY: Macmillan.book series.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerry Stahl.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stahl, G. From Intersubjectivity to Group Cognition. Comput Supported Coop Work 25, 355–384 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-016-9243-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-016-9243-z

Key words

Navigation